Which element of fitness requires sensory input from the eyes, ears, and proprioceptors?

  • Journal List
  • J Neurophysiol
  • PMC6734411

J Neurophysiol. 2019 Aug 1; 122(2): 525–538.

Control of Movement

Abstract

In addition to being a prerequisite for many activities of daily living, the ability to maintain steady upright standing is a relevant model to study sensorimotor integrative function. Upright standing requires managing multimodal sensory inputs to produce finely tuned motor output that can be adjusted to accommodate changes in standing conditions and environment. The sensory information used for postural control mainly arises from the vestibular system of the inner ear, vision, and proprioception. Proprioception (sense of body position and movement) encompasses signals from mechanoreceptors (proprioceptors) located in muscles, tendons, and joint capsules. There is general agreement that proprioception signals from leg muscles provide the primary source of information for postural control. This is because of their exquisite sensitivity to detect body sway during unperturbed upright standing that mainly results from variations in leg muscle length induced by rotations around the ankle joint. However, aging is associated with alterations of muscle spindles and their neural pathways, which induce a decrease in the sensitivity, acuity, and integration of the proprioceptive signal. These alterations promote changes in postural control that reduce its efficiency and thereby may have deleterious consequences for the functional independence of an individual. This narrative review provides an overview of how aging alters the proprioceptive signal from the legs and presents compelling evidence that these changes modify the neural control of upright standing.

Keywords: balance, Ia afferents, muscle spindles, proprioception, upright standing

INTRODUCTION

Among hominid species, the natural bipedal erected posture is one of the most remarkable biomechanical characteristics of human beings. Upright standing in humans can be modeled as an inverted pendulum rotating around the ankle joint, with the intended equilibrium position being a slight forward tilt of the body, generating a gravity-driven instability. Because passive ankle stiffness cannot compensate for the forward-toppling torque (Loram and Lakie 2002), an active modulation of the neural drive to the motor neuron pools innervating the ankle plantar flexor muscles is necessary to adjust ankle stiffness. Its complexity (multijoint system) and its inherent instability (forward tilt of the body) make the upright standing posture a relevant model to study sensorimotor integration, as it requires managing multimodal sensory inputs to produce finely tuned motor output depending on the environment. The ability to maintain steady upright standing (referred to in this review as postural control) has long been considered an automatic task mainly controlled by spinal and brain stem structures. However, it is now clear that postural control also involves cortical area structures adjusting the motor commands as the state of the body and the environment change (Lephart et al. 1997). The sensory information used for postural control mainly arises from the vestibular system of the inner ear, vision, and proprioception (Peterka 2002). Because of the lower proprioceptive threshold for the perception of body sway during upright standing compared with visual and vestibular systems (Fitzpatrick and McCloskey 1994), proprioceptive inputs play a critical role in postural control (Doumas et al. 2008; Speers et al. 2002; Teasdale and Simoneau 2001; Van Impe et al. 2012).

In addition to revealing the capacity of the nervous system to face such a sensorimotor challenge, investigation of postural control also allows for determination of key factors for prevention and rehabilitation interventions. In this context, aging is accompanied by alterations in the proprioceptive system (Shaffer and Harrison 2007) that should contribute to changes in postural control (Anson et al. 2017; Horak et al. 1989). Considering the increasing percentage of the world population over the age of 60 yr, a better understanding of the effect of age on leg proprioception and its implications in postural control is of paramount relevance. Therefore, our main objective in this review is to provide an overview of how aging alters the proprioceptive signal from leg muscles and to put forward compelling evidence that these changes modify the neural control of upright standing.

LEG PROPRIOCEPTION IN POSTURAL CONTROL

Proprioception encompasses signals from mechanoreceptors—transducers that convert mechanical stimuli into action potentials—located in muscles, tendons, and joint capsules (proprioceptors); information from cutaneous mechanoreceptors (cutaneous stretch receptor) associated with tactile sensations is considered as additional sensory sources that complete proprioceptive inputs (Riemann and Lephart 2002). Proprioception plays a critical role in movement control by providing inputs to internal models that couple sensory signals and motor commands (Wolpert and Kawato 1998).

One important component of postural control is the ability to detect body sway that, in upright standing, mainly results in variations of leg muscle length induced by rotations around the ankle joint (Di Giulio et al. 2009; Fitzpatrick et al. 1994). Because of their in-series arrangement with muscle fibers, Golgi tendon organs are ideally located to encode variations in the force developed by the contracting muscle fibers and contribute to the senses of force and heaviness (Proske and Gandevia 2012). However, such anatomical position does not provide Golgi tendon organs with the possibility to encode changes in muscle length (Macefield 2005), thereby reducing their ability to provide relevant information on limb position and joint movement. Feedback from joint receptors appears to provide information restricted to extreme joint position (Macefield 2005) and is therefore unlikely to play a large role in postural control. Finally, cutaneous inputs from the plantar surface of the foot have very weak effects, if any, on postural control during unperturbed upright standing. A meta-analysis underscored that cooling the plantar surface of the foot had a very weak, nonsignificant, effect on upright standing (Hoch and Russell 2016). Accordingly, even though proprioceptive signals originate from multiple mechanoreceptors, there is a general agreement that muscle spindle receptors provide the primary source of proprioceptive information for postural control.

Furthermore, proprioceptive inputs trigger the rapid, automatic, and coordinated postural responses to unexpected movement of a support surface. In agreement, Stapley et al. (2002) suggested that the large afferent fibers (Ia afferents) are critical for the timing of automatic postural responses to ensure coordinated control of the body center of mass and balance after unexpected disturbances of the support surface. In contrast, the timing of automatic postural responses was unaffected by loss of vestibular information after bilateral labyrinthectomy, even when vision was absent (Inglis and Macpherson 1995). In addition, during a fall, individuals prepare for the impact based on sensory information, which would be mainly of proprioceptive origin at the fall onset (Le Goic et al. 2018). Therefore, alterations within the proprioceptive signal likely increase the risk of falls and impede the ability to reduce fall-related injury.

Muscle Proprioception

Muscle spindles are sensors that consist of intrafusal muscle fibers enclosed in a sheath, fusiform in shape (spindle), and arranged in parallel to the extrafusal muscle fibers, rendering them very sensitive to muscle length and its rate of change. Primary muscle spindle afferents, referred to as group Ia fibers, terminate in the annulospiral ending around the central part of the bag 1, bag 2, and chain intrafusal fibers. Secondary muscle spindle endings, referred to as group II fibers, supply the bag 2 and chain fibers (Proske and Gregory 2002) (Fig. 1). Although both primary and secondary endings act as stretch receptors, the primary ending has a higher dynamic sensitivity. Accordingly, the discharges of primary endings convey information about changes in muscle length and velocity, whereas those of secondary endings mainly carry information about changes in muscle length (McCloskey 1978). Muscle spindles, widely scattered in the muscle belly, are stretched when the muscle lengthens, which results in the generation of action potentials on afferent fibers that form synapses with alpha motor neurons and interneurons or convey proprioceptive information to the sensorimotor cortex and the cerebellum (Fig. 2A).

Which element of fitness requires sensory input from the eyes, ears, and proprioceptors?

Schematic representation of sensory and fusimotor innervation of the intrafusal fibers of a muscle spindle, with the exception of α fibers that only innervate extrafusal fibers, in contrast to β fibers that innervate both extrafusal and intrafusal (bag 1) fibers. Arrows indicate the direction of the impulse conduction. Sensory afferent axons Ia and II convey information to sensory neurons located in the dorsal root ganglia.

Which element of fitness requires sensory input from the eyes, ears, and proprioceptors?

A: schematic illustration of the spinal and ascending pathways conveying proprioceptive information. Arrows indicate the direction of flow of the neural information. B: effects of aging on the proprioceptive system. Morphological changes in muscle spindles and parent afferent fibers decrease the acuity of the proprioceptive signal. The combination of changes in afferent fibers and spinal network alters the efficacy of synaptic transmission of proprioceptive volleys from muscle spindles. Spinal and supraspinal changes alter the neural integration of the inputs from muscle spindles. EPSPs, excitatory postsynaptic potentials; MNs, motoneurons; ↑, increase; ↓, decrease or alteration.

Gamma motor neurons provide motor innervation of intrafusal fibers to modulate the sensitivity of muscle spindles to muscle length changes. These neurons are termed “fusimotor” neurons (Matthews 2011), with γ dynamic and γ static fusimotor fibers. The fusimotor drive (gamma motor neurons), under the control of descending tract (Fig. 2A), is assumed to modulate the dynamic and static sensitivity of muscle spindles with the task requirements (Prochazka 1989). Beta motor neurons, which are smaller and less abundant than other motor neuron subtypes, innervate both intrafusal and extrafusal muscle fibers (Bessou et al. 1965).

Even though muscle spindles respond unidirectionally across the entire physiological range of movement (Burgess et al. 1982; Macefield et al. 1990), interpretation of the information conveyed by muscle spindle afferents from plantar flexor muscles during upright standing can be confounded by the fact that length changes of the muscle-tendon unit are transmitted indirectly via compliant tendinous tissue. On example of such potential bias is provided by the uncoupling between changes in the length of the muscle-tendon unit of gastrocnemius medialis and muscle fascicles, and presumably muscle spindles (Baudry et al. 2012; Loram et al. 2004). This suggests that the plantar flexor muscles may not be the main contributors to proprioceptive signals during upright standing. In contrast, the tibialis anterior, which is mainly quiescent in standing, could provide more relevant proprioceptive inputs. Di Giulio et al. (2009) and Day et al. (2013) indicated that changes in the length of fascicles of tibialis anterior were tightly coupled with changes in sway position. Further investigations revealed that muscle spindles in the human tibialis anterior encode fascicle length of parent muscles during passive length changes (Day et al. 2017). In agreement, Aniss et al. (1990) already showed that muscle spindle afferents from the tibialis anterior can provide information on backward sway direction.

Nonetheless, a recent study (Peters et al. 2017) investigating the coding of ankle angle and velocity by human calf muscle spindles indicated that activity of muscle spindle afferents reflected passive ankle movements at frequencies and amplitudes similar to those recorded during upright standing. Even though voluntary contraction of calf muscles reduced spindle sensitivity to such ankle movements, Peters et al. (2017) showed that muscle spindles remained sensitive enough to provide valuable sensory feedback for postural control. Furthermore, in an elegant study, Blum et al. (2017) underscored that muscle-tendon force and its first time derivative predicted the transient instantaneous firing rate of muscle spindles in anesthetized cats. This suggests that signal from muscle spindles could be involved in coding force variations within the muscles, such as those associated with mechanical perturbations of the body. For an up-to-date review on this topic, the reader is invited to read Proske and Allen (2019).

Relevance of Leg Proprioception in Postural Control

The significant role of leg proprioception in postural control is highlighted in patients with polyneuropathies that compromise proprioception. Such individuals exhibit greater center of pressure excursions compared with control subjects (Bergin et al. 1995). Similar observations were reported for patients with diabetic peripheral neuropathy. In response to a translation of the support surface, diabetic patients showed a delayed onset of muscle activity and an impaired ability to scale torque magnitude to both the velocity and amplitude of surface translations (Inglis et al. 1994). The role of muscle spindles in postural control was also assessed experimentally by altering the proprioceptive signal. This can be done, for example, by using pneumatic cuffs around limbs to progressively block action potential propagation in afferent axons. When the cuffs were placed bilaterally at the ankle level, the imposed ischemia had relatively little influence on upright standing. When the cuffs were placed above the knee, however, ischemia increased antero-posterior excursion of the center of pressure, indicating that leg proprioceptors are more relevant to postural control than stretch-sensitive receptors of the sole of the foot and proprioceptive signals from foot muscles (Knellwolf et al. 2019; Mauritz and Dietz 1980). Muscle proprioception can also be altered by vibrating tendons, which activates muscle spindle primary endings and produces a sensation of displacement of the associated body segment (Burke et al. 1976; Goodwin et al. 1972; Roll and Vedel 1982). When a person stands upright with his/her eyes closed, the vibration of the Achilles tendons generates a backward shift of the center of pressure to counteract the illusion of falling forward (Eklund 1972). An opposite response occurs when vibration is applied to the distal tendon of the tibialis anterior muscle (Kavounoudias et al. 1999). Eysel-Gosepath et al. (2016) observed a greater excursion of the center of pressure during Achilles tendon vibration compared with a situation in which subjects closed their eyes, suggesting that muscle proprioception is more important for postural control than vision in healthy young adults. Furthermore, the backward lean observed during Achilles tendon vibration was sustained after the cessation of tendon vibration and accompanied by trunk extension, posterior tilt of the pelvis, and flexion of the hips and knees (Thompson et al. 2007). These results indicate that prolonged perturbation of proprioceptive input from leg muscles modifies the perception of the body’s vertical position that alters postural control.

Overall, these different approaches underscore the key role of leg proprioception in postural control. Changes in postural control observed in older adults could therefore reflect, in part, alterations in muscle spindles and their neural pathways.

Aging is associated with numerous changes in the neuromuscular system (Hunter et al. 2016; Shaffer and Harrison 2007) that are accompanied by a general decline in motor performance, as reflected in a decrease in maximal muscle force (Frontera et al. 1991) and force control accuracy (Baudry et al. 2010; Tracy and Enoka 2002), and an increase in center of pressure excursions during unperturbed upright standing (Abrahamová and Hlavačka 2008; Laughton et al. 2003; Nagai et al. 2011; Van Impe et al. 2013). From a clinical point of view, it is worth noting that the decrease in postural control is associated with an increased risk of falling (Horak 2006; Maki et al. 1994). Alterations in muscle spindles and their afferents, along with the integration of the signal at the supraspinal level, have been shown to influence proprioceptive perception and postural control in older adults (Goble et al. 2011, 2012).

Muscle Spindles

As shown in Fig. 2B and described below, aging alters both the structures and the functioning of the proprioceptive system.

Morphology and innervation.

Swash and Fox (1972) reported an increase in the capsular thickness of muscle spindles with age in postmortem human muscles, accompanied by a slight decrease in the mean number of intrafusal muscle fibers, whereas Kararizou et al. (2005) reported that the diameter of the spindles decreases with age. Liu et al. (2005) reported an age-related decrease in the total number of intrafusal muscle fibers and chain fibers in postmortem human muscle spindles, whereas no difference was observed for the bag fibers. In addition, Kim et al. (2007) showed that in aged rats primary endings lost their typical annulospiral configuration, becoming tapered and irregular in shape. A comparable alteration was not observed for secondary endings.

Looking at the innervation of muscle spindles, an early study (Swallow 1966) performed on human cadavers (aged between 16 and 82 yr) reported that in the anterior tibial nerve of the foot the total number of nerve fibers drastically decreased with age, with a significant decrease in the proportion of large fibers in the older subjects. As Ia afferents are the largest peripheral axons, this suggests an age-related decrease in the amount of muscle spindle afferents, especially the Ia afferents. More recently, Vaughan et al. (2017) confirmed in mice that proprioceptive sensory neurons degenerate with aging and that this degeneration starts earlier than atrophy of the intrafusal muscle fibers.

Sensitivity.

When investigating the static and dynamic sensitivities of muscle spindle primary endings, Miwa et al. (1995) reported a lower discharge frequency in response to muscle stretch (dynamic sensitivity) in aged rats, whereas the static sensitivity did not exhibit an age-related effect. Kim et al. (2007) also observed a loss of dynamic sensitivity. The dampening of muscle spindle sensitivity seen with aging may be accounted for by the morphological changes discussed above.

During tendon vibration, the repetitive activity of Ia afferents decreases the Hoffmann reflex amplitude, which assesses the net excitatory input of group I afferents onto spinal motor neurons. However, Burke et al. (1996) reported that tendon vibration has less of an effect on the Hoffmann reflex in older compared with younger adults. Age-related changes in the effectiveness of the synaptic transmission through pre- and postsynaptic mechanisms likely contribute to these observations. The deleterious morphological changes of aged muscle spindles that decrease the dynamic sensitivity of muscle spindles could also reduce the vibration-related muscle spindle activity. In agreement, Chung et al. (2005) reported weaker and slower reflex-induced muscle force generation in response to Achilles tendon tap in old compared with young adults.

Signal Integration

Signal integration is referred to here as the summation, gating, and modulation of varying combinations of excitatory and inhibitory synaptic inputs distributed throughout the central nervous system.

Conduction velocity.

Conduction velocity (Boxer et al. 1988; Kim et al. 2007) and axon diameter shift in the direction of slower speeds and smaller axons, abolishing differences between primary and secondary endings with aging (Kim et al. 2007). Combined with the reduction in the conduction velocity of motor axons (Morales et al. 1987), the decrease in conduction velocity of Ia afferents should contribute to increasing the latency of reflex responses originating from the muscle spindle pathway. In support of this assumption, the latency of the Hoffmann reflex in the soleus (Baudry et al. 2015; Sabbahi and Sedgwick 1982; Scaglioni et al. 2003) and stretch reflex evoked in the tibialis anterior muscle increases with age (Klass et al. 2011).

Spinal synaptic integration.

When looking at the integration of the muscle spindle signal at the spinal level, one should consider the spinal interneuron networks. Terao et al. (1996) demonstrated that the number of small neurons in the intermediate zone of the ventral horn decreased with aging. As those small neurons are thought to be mostly interneurons, these results suggest a decreased complexity in the spinal network that may alter the integration of the afferent signal. In addition, older cats exhibit longer rise time and half-width of the Ia excitatory postsynaptic potentials (EPSPs) compared with younger cats, accompanied by a lesser rate of rise of EPSPs (Boxer et al. 1988; Chase et al. 1985). The smaller rate of rise of Ia-induced EPSPs in old cats likely decreases their efficacy to promote motor neuron discharge (Fetz and Gustafsson 1983).

The age-related reduction in the effectiveness of the Ia afferents to activate motor neurons could also be influenced by changes in Ia presynaptic inhibition. Butchart et al. (1993) suggested a decrease in Ia presynaptic inhibition as inferred from a smaller reduction in soleus Hoffmann reflex in response to tendon vibration in older adults. In contrast, Morita et al. (1995), in the same muscle, suggested an increase in Ia presynaptic inhibition in older adults based on the reduced facilitation of the Hoffmann reflex to a conditioning stimulation applied on heteronymous Ia afferents. Finally, when Ia presynaptic inhibition was assessed with two complementary Hoffmann reflex conditioning techniques, the amount of Ia presynaptic inhibition did not differ between young and older adults in forearm (extensor carpi radialis) and leg (soleus) muscle (Baudry et al. 2010; Baudry and Duchateau 2012). These divergent results do not speak in favor of a change in Ia presynaptic inhibition with aging but may reflect a decrease in the number of Ia afferents and/or in their conduction velocities.

Supraspinal integration.

Although little is known about the role of aging in the integration of the proprioceptive signal at the supraspinal level, there is evidence that older adults experiencing mobility impairment are more likely to have underlying alterations in the structure and function of the brain (Kilgour et al. 2014).

structural changes.

Efficient integration of different sensory inputs in the brain might be compromised because of age-related declines in white (Abe et al. 2002; Sullivan and Pfefferbaum 2006) and gray (Good et al. 2001; Kalpouzos et al. 2009) matter integrity. In agreement, fractional anisotropy, a measure of white matter integrity, of frontal and fronto-occipital tracts was predictive of postural performance in older but not young adults (Van Impe et al. 2012). The decrease in brain gray matter thickness in pre- and postcentral gyrus areas (Good et al. 2001; Salat et al. 2004), which are related to the sensorimotor regions of the brain, may also result in poorer proprioceptive integration. Furthermore, neuronal loss in the pallidum, a relevant region for postural control, has been associated with difficulties in holding the semitandem standing position (Rosano et al. 2007), a position that likely requires greater proprioceptive inputs compared with normal bipedal (hip width) foot position (Sarabon et al. 2013). Similarly, age-related decline in the brain stem structure partly accounts for the decline in postural control (Boisgontier et al. 2017).

brain activation.

Several elements support age-related changes in brain activation in relation to upright standing. As observed during nonpostural motor tasks, (Heuninckx et al. 2005, 2008), older adults exhibit increased activity in somatosensory cortices (right postcentral gyrus) during motor imagery of upright stance compared with young adults (Zwergal et al. 2012). Similarly, Mouthon et al. (2018) reported a greater activity in the supplementary motor area, motor area, premotor cortex, and putamen of older adults during motor imagery of upright standing in various conditions compared with young adults. This engagement of additional cortical areas most likely reflects a compensatory mechanism for age-related sensorimotor decline (Heuninckx et al. 2008; Reuter-Lorenz and Lustig 2005). Changes in brain neurochemistry that occur with aging can also induce motor deficits (for a review, see Seidler et al. 2010). Serotonin concentration, for example, is lower in older compared with young adults, especially in the cingulate cortex and the putamen (Gottfries 1990). Interestingly, Goble et al. (2012) reported a positive relation between the sense of joint position—which mainly rests on muscle spindle inputs (Proske and Gandevia 2012)—and the neural activity in the right putamen. Reduced brain activation in this region in response to proprioceptive stimulation could reflect a loss of proprioceptive integration in older adults. In this vein, Piitulainen et al. (2018) reported that cortical processing of the proprioceptive signal is altered by aging. Furthermore, Ozdemir et al. (2018) indicated that impairments in perceptual processing of sensory signals contribute to prolong muscle response delays during perturbed upright standing in older adults.

One potential consequence of these structural and functional changes is an increase in neural noise within the sensorimotor system that may impair the neural signal (Cremer and Zeef 1987; Kail 1997; Mozolic et al. 2012). The neural noise hypothesis rests on the assumption that the effective signal-to-noise ratio decreases (Cremer and Zeef 1987) because of increased spontaneous/baseline neural spiking activity (Hong and Rebec 2012), which disrupts the fidelity of neural signals. A lower signal-to-noise ratio should require greater processing that may partly explain the greater brain activation observed in older adults. When considering the decrease in sensitivity and transmission of the proprioceptive signal due to age-related alterations in muscle spindles and their neural pathways (see above), an increase in neural noise should further challenge the integration of proprioceptive signals and contribute to decreasing the relevance of the proprioceptive signals for postural control in older adults.

HOW IMPAIRED LEG PROPRIOCEPTION CHANGES POSTURAL CONTROL IN OLDER ADULTS

The alterations in muscle spindles and their neural pathways with aging encompass among other mechanisms decreases in the sensitivity, acuity, and integration of the proprioceptive signal. The framework of the potential links between age-related changes in leg proprioception and postural control in older adults is synthetized in Fig. 3 and further discussed below.

Which element of fitness requires sensory input from the eyes, ears, and proprioceptors?

Framework of how age-related alteration of leg proprioception changes postural control and its adaptability. ↑, increase; ↓, decrease or alteration.

Behavioral Aspects

Different approaches were used to investigate whether age-related changes in proprioception are associated with reduced postural control. Relevant for the following discussion is the fact that in most of the studies mentioned, an increase in body sway amplitude (assessed through kinematics of the center of mass), center of pressure excursions (assessed by ground reaction forces), or trunk acceleration (assessed through accelerometers) are assumed to underscore alterations in postural control. However, recent advancements in postural control bring forward that increased excursions of the center of mass or pressure in older adults can be interpreted as a positive adaptation to ensure that sensory inputs exceed the thresholds for detection and enhance the sensory information available to the central nervous system (Carpenter et al. 2010; Rajachandrakumar et al. 2018). Along these lines, the increase in body sway or center of pressure excursions may reflect an increase in sensory thresholds (reduced sensitivity) and/or reduced integration capacity. Nonetheless, regardless of the interpretation and the metrics used, these changes are evidence of an overall decline of the postural system that likely rests, in part, on a decreased ability to detect body sway.

An interesting approach to assess the age-related changes in proprioception during upright standing consists of using tendon vibration of plantar flexor muscles (see Relevance of Leg Proprioception in Postural Control). Most studies report a lesser influence of Achilles tendon vibration on postural control in older compared with young adults (Ehsani et al. 2018; Hay et al. 1996; Nakagawa et al. 1993; Penzer et al. 2015; Pyykkö et al. 1990; Quoniam et al. 1995; Toosizadeh et al. 2018). For example, Pyykkö and colleagues (Pyykkö et al. 1990) assessed a proprioceptive ratio that merely represents the weight assigned to ankle proprioceptive signals to regulate postural control. The ratio was calculated between sway velocities during baseline stance (no vibrating stimulus) and during Achilles tendon vibration at 80 Hz, with both baseline and vibration conditions being performed with eyes closed. The greater quotient in older (0.84) than younger (0.51) adults indicates that tendon vibrations have less of an effect on postural control in older adults. Using another approach, Penzer et al. (2015) assessed postural control before and after long-duration Achilles tendon vibration (1 h, 80 Hz). The 1-h tendon vibration altered postural control in young but not older adults. The decreased effect of vibration with aging suggests a lesser reliance on leg proprioception for postural control in older adults. The lower vibration-induced postural perturbation observed in older adults can also reflect, at least in part, a decreased sensitivity of muscle spindles to vibration (see Sensitivity).

However, other studies reported no age effect or greater vibration-induced perturbations in older adults (Abrahámová et al. 2009; Brumagne et al. 2004; Ito et al. 2018; Maitre et al. 2013). Abrahámová et al. (2009) reported greater vibration effects on postural control in older adults for tendon vibration. Remarkably, the greater effects of vibration were accompanied by differences in postural strategy (body stiffening) adopted by older adults in response to vibration. These results suggest that responses to vibration may depend on postural strategy, a strategy that can be influenced by the level of force that has to be produced by the plantar flexors to maintain upright standing. Indeed, vibration effects decreased in contracted muscles (Ansems et al. 2006; Eklund 1972; Goodwin et al. 1972; McCloskey 1973). This may be particularly relevant when considering the greater activation of plantar flexor muscles in older compared with young adults during upright standing (Billot et al. 2010). In addition, other studies showing greater postural perturbation in older adults in response to vibration used vibration frequency of ≤60 Hz. As vibration effects increase with frequencies (Roll and Vedel 1982), low vibration frequencies may have limited the effects of vibration in young adults, thereby reducing the age-related difference in vibration-induced postural perturbation. Furthermore, low-frequency vibrations (<60 Hz) maximally activate Meissner’s corpuscles (Martin and Jessel 1991) and can perturb sensory weighting between cutaneous stretch receptor and muscle proprioceptors (Pavailler et al. 2016) more in older adults who experience altered proprioceptive processing. Overall, if the use of tendon vibration tends to indicate a lesser contribution of leg proprioception to postural control in older compared with young adults, further work is needed to draw a definitive conclusion.

Another relevant approach to highlight the influence of altered leg proprioception in postural control is to investigate the relation between postural control and the integrity of the proprioceptive system. Along this line, Lord et al. (1991) searched for a relation between joint position sense and postural control in a sample of 95 older adults (mean age: 82.7 yr). The authors found that poor joint position sense was associated with larger body sway. With a bigger sample (550 women, aged between 20 and 99 yr), Lord and Ward (1994) confirmed that proprioception was a predicting factor of postural control in standing on a firm surface with eyes closed. Deshpande et al. (2016) investigated the relation between ankle joint proprioception, measured as the threshold for perception of passive movement that is generated by primary endings of muscle spindles, and postural control in a population-based sample across the adult life span (n = 790). The authors reported that poor proprioception was consistently associated with poor postural control, with proprioception acuity decreasing with age. By using multivariate kinematics and joint torque measurements during dynamic posturography, Speers et al. (2002) found that the increase in body sway amplitude with age was partly related to an increase in sensory noise that reduces the ability to detect small body motions through proprioceptive signals. Recently, Anson and colleagues (2017) assessed the relations between visual, vestibular, and proprioceptive functions and age with postural sway in 366 subjects aged between 20 and 103 yr. Multiple linear regressions indicated that proprioceptive function was the best predictor of sway area, whereas age per se was not a consistent predictor of sway characteristics. Accordingly, the authors concluded that loss of peripheral sensory function explains much of the age-related decrease in postural control. Together, these results underscore that age-related impairments in detection and processing of proprioceptive signals alter postural control.

Neural Aspects

Increase in coactivation of lower limb muscles.

To adjust the mechanical properties of a limb, the central nervous system can simultaneously coactivate agonist and antagonist muscles around a joint. Increasing the level of coactivation augments joint impedance (Hogan 1984) and thereby provides better joint mechanical stability. Antagonist coactivation may also counteract, or at least mitigate, responses to external perturbations and forces due to limb dynamics and gravity (Finley et al. 2012). The nervous system can therefore utilize a feedforward strategy relying on antagonist coactivation rather than feedback mechanisms, especially short-latency reflex responses, to increase ankle stability during unperturbed upright standing.

Compared with young adults, older adults maintain upright standing with a greater coactivation level between the plantar flexors and dorsiflexors of the ankle, especially when upright standing or sensory conditions are challenged (Baudry and Duchateau 2012; Benjuya et al. 2004; Donath et al. 2015; Melzer et al. 2001). Furthermore, previous work (Baudry and Duchateau 2012; Nagai et al. 2011) reported that muscle coactivation was significantly higher in older adults exhibiting poorer postural control. The increase in coactivation in older adults partly reflects a compensatory mechanism to changes in proprioception (Manchester et al. 1989; Nagai et al. 2011). In agreement, greater coactivation was observed in deafferented patients than in healthy subjects (Sainburg et al. 1995). Moreover, age-related alterations in peripheral sensory perception contribute to increase antagonistic leg muscle coactivation (Ortega and Farley 2015). The level of coactivation has also been inversely associated with the amount of Ia presynaptic inhibition of muscle spindle afferents converging onto soleus motor neuron pools during upright standing (Baudry and Duchateau 2012), suggesting that reductions of Ia afferent input onto soleus are associated with a greater level of coactivation. However, if coactivation may reflect a strategy of stiffening and freezing the lower legs to control upright standing (Benjuya et al. 2004), it could be inefficient to improve postural control (Latash 2018), and thereby uselessly increases the energetic cost of postural control. Associated with the fact that a greater reliance on feedforward strategies would decrease automatic processing of postural control (Finley et al. 2012), the increase of coactivation in response to age-related alterations in leg proprioception may not be an efficient strategy for postural control.

Increased dependence on visual information.

As the information carried by each sensory source is weighted depending on the current functional state of the source, the postural task, and the context in which it is performed, the most reliable sensory inputs are emphasized and the less reliable inputs are weakened. A decrease in the reliability of proprioceptive information should be compensated, therefore, by upweighting another sensory source such as visual information. This assumption is supported by experimental data obtained in situations in which sensory conflict was generated through bilateral Achilles tendon vibration and contrasting visual flow (Kabbaligere et al. 2017). The results indicated a sensory reweighting process directly proportional to the relative reliability of the cues. In this study, the reduced weighting of the proprioceptive cues was compensated by visual cues. Along these lines, declines in peripheral sensory perception with aging have been involved in elevated reliance on visual feedback (Franz et al. 2015; Jeka et al. 2010). In agreement, numerous studies underscore that older adults tend to rely more upon their visual input than other sensory systems to control upright standing (Borger et al. 1999; Simoneau et al. 1999; Wade et al. 1995; Yeh et al. 2014). However, less work has focused on the potential link between such an increase in the relevance of visual inputs and impairment in proprioceptive input. Sundermier et al. (1996) investigated the influence of visual flow from a moving visual surrounding on postural control in young adults, healthy older adults, and older adults with balance problems. The group of older adults with balance problems had greater center of pressure excursions when changing the visual flow, indicating an overreliance on visual cues for posture control. The authors suggest that this greater reliance on visual cues reflects borderline somatosensory deficits, as this group had subclinical indications for somatosensory impairments and brain changes. With another experimental approach, Haibach et al. (2009) reported greater postural motion in older compared with young adults in response to the oscillation of a virtual moving room. In this experiment, older adults had an increased egomotion (increased body sway in response to visual scene motion) but a decreased vection (decreased perception of movement) in response to visual scene motion. The authors claim that the reduction in proprioceptive inputs that accompanies aging leads to an increased amplitude of body sways before people perceive visual scene motion, reflecting somehow a greater reliance on visual information. Furthermore, Eikema and colleagues (Eikema et al. 2012) indicated that older adults show less sensory reweighting in quiet standing because of a greater visual field dependence. An additional parameter that should lead to greater reliance on visual information in relation with poorer proprioceptive input could be muscle weakness. Indeed, individuals with lower limb weakness rely more on vision to detect and stabilize their body sway than people with strong lower limb muscles (Butler et al. 2008). This may be explained by the fact that the increase in muscle activation required to maintain upright standing with aging (Billot et al. 2010) may reduce the proprioceptive acuity (Proske et al. 2000; Wise et al. 1998; see also Behavioral Aspects). It is noteworthy that strategies incorporating visual information induce delayed and less accurate fall avoidance responses, in contrast to adaptative strategies based on proprioceptive information (Vouriot et al. 2004).

A critical examination of these results, however, raises two comments. First, previous work on manual (Boisgontier et al. 2014) and postural (Berard et al. 2012; Eikema et al. 2012; Jeka et al. 2010; O’Connor et al. 2008) tasks suggests that older adults may experience more difficulty in suppressing unreliable visual cues. In addition, Bugnariu and Fung (2007) showed that aging alters the interaction of the somatosensory and visual systems in the control of balance and the ability to resolve sensory conflicts. These elements may challenge the interpretation that greater visually evoked postural responses purely reflect an increased reliance on vision to control upright standing in older adults. Second, the general decrease in postural control when visual inputs are distorted or occluded may indicate that poor proprioception cannot compensate for changes in visual conditions. If such an assumption nuances an increased reliance on visual information due to impaired proprioception per se, it nevertheless indicates that altered proprioception makes older adults more dependent on visual information to control upright standing (Lord and Ward 1994), thereby reducing the flexibility of the sensory reweighting process (Jeka et al. 2010; Wade et al. 1995). Overall, most studies point to an upweighting of visual information to compensate for age-related impairments in leg proprioception.

Increase in controlled processing of upright standing.

Motor control ranges from totally controlled processing that involves basal ganglia-cortical loops (Jacobs and Horak 2007) to automatic processing involving spinal and brain stem networks (Honeycutt et al. 2009). An alteration in proprioceptive input from muscle spindles likely reduces the efficacy of the automatic processing, thereby increasing the controlled processing and the cognitive load associated with postural control. In agreement, proprioception loss, as observed in deafferented patients, leads to an increase in the cognitive resources needed to perform even simple movements (Ingram et al. 2000). Accordingly, older adults increase the attentional resources dedicated to postural control, as revealed by the dual-task paradigm (Baudry and Gaillard 2014; Berger and Bernard-Demanze 2011; Boisgontier and Nougier 2013; Gaillardin and Baudry 2018; Rankin et al. 2000; Teasdale and Simoneau 2001; Woollacott and Shumway-Cook 2002).

Additional elements in favor of a shift to a more controlled process are provided by transcranial magnetic stimulation (TMS), which allows one to assess changes in the excitability of the motor corticospinal pathways that encompass cortical and spinal motor neurons as well as spinal interneurons with task requirements (Petersen et al. 2003). For example, the motor evoked potential elicited in the soleus by TMS increased from seated or supported bipedal posture to normal bipedal posture (Soto et al. 2006; Tokuno et al. 2009), and motor evoked potential amplitude was greater in old compared with young adults during upright standing (Baudry et al. 2014a). Furthermore, when the excitability of the corticomotoneuronal pathway, which provides a more direct access to cortical excitability, was assessed, older adults also exhibited greater corticomotoneuronal excitability than young adults during upright standing (Baudry et al. 2014b). Interestingly, a positive association between corticomotoneuronal excitability and soleus muscle activity indicates that the individuals who activated the soleus muscle to a greater extent were those who had greater corticomotoneuronal excitability. This should be considered in regard to the opposite relation between Hoffmann reflex amplitude and soleus muscle activity during upright standing (Baudry 2016; see below) and to the fact that large muscle activity may alter the proprioceptive signal. The silent period observed in response to TMS, reflecting the excitability of intracortical inhibitory networks (Orth and Rothwell 2004), was depressed in older adults during upright standing, thereby suggesting a decrease in intracortical inhibition (Baudry et al. 2015). To go further in the modulation of intracortical inhibitory circuits, Papegaaij et al. (2016) investigated the age-related changes in motor cortical activity during nonpostural and postural contractions with varying levels of postural challenge by TMS-induced electromyography depression. Even though age does not affect the motor control strategy of modulating motor cortical activity with increasing postural challenge, the motor cortical modulation appears at a lower task difficulty with increasing age. These results likely reflect a more controlled processing of upright standing that may be in part due to changes in muscle spindle afferent signals (Heuninckx et al. 2004, 2005). In agreement, a recent study suggested that impaired proprioception contributes to alter the cortical control of upright standing in older adults (Ozdemir et al. 2018).

The amplitude of the Hoffmann reflex is depressed in the soleus during normal bipedal posture compared with seated or supported bipedal posture (Koceja et al. 1993; Tokuno et al. 2009) and from stable to unstable upright standing conditions (Earles et al. 2000). When measuring the amplitude of the Hoffmann reflex in seated and upright standing conditions in individuals aged between 19 and 76 yr, with the soleus background activity being matched between the two positions, Baudry et al. (2015) observed a decrease in Hoffmann reflex amplitude from seated to standing regardless of age. However, the reduction in Hoffmann reflex amplitude induced by upright standing increased with age, more so in individuals over the age of 60 yr compared with younger individuals. Because subjects had similar levels of habitual physical activity, such results cannot be explained by an age-related increase in sedentary lifestyle that could induce a more pronounced decrease in Hoffmann reflex amplitude (Chalmers and Knutzen 2000). One interpretation of the greater posture-related decrease in Hoffmann reflex amplitude with aging could rest on a reduced ability of the proprioceptive system to provide an appropriate signal to activate leg muscles. This should reduce the automatic processing associated with upright standing.

Together, these different approaches and viewpoints emphasize the impact of age-related deterioration of proprioception on postural control, as synthesized in Fig. 3. Because the aging process cannot be stopped, future research should be oriented to interventions designed to limit alterations of the proprioceptive system. Without being exhaustive, we highlight in the next section some promising approaches, going from basic behaviors (nontargeted physical activity) to more sophisticated interventions (augmented sensory feedback), that may counteract the aging process of proprioception and its consequences on postural control.

PERSPECTIVES

As postural control and gait contribute to healthy aging (Lara et al. 2013), it thereby requires maintaining the capacity of the proprioceptive system because, as developed in the previous sections, impaired proprioception is associated with poor postural control in older adults (Anson et al. 2017; Deshpande et al. 2016; Goble et al. 2011; Lord et al. 1991; Toledo and Barela 2010). The following approaches appear to be promising to preserve proprioceptive function and postural control with aging.

Regular physical activity has been proposed to attenuate the decline in proprioception in older adults (Adamo et al. 2009; Ribeiro and Oliveira 2010; Petrella et al. 1997). For example, by comparing young, active older, and sedentary older adults, Petrella et al. (2017) observed a decrease in proprioception with age but brought forward the positive role of regular activity to attenuate this decline. In addition to maintaining proprioceptive acuity, Maitre et al. (2013) indicated that regular physical activity may preserve the ability to reweight sensory sources for a better use of sensory information. These studies indicate that nontargeted physical activity can preserve proprioceptive acuity and postural control in older adults and suggest that regular exercise may represent a strategy to reduce the incidence of poor proprioception with aging. However, this approach does not allow for the determination of a link between specific exercises and improvement in proprioception. In fact, there is only limited evidence on the trainability of the proprioceptive sense and the extent to which improvements in proprioceptive function impact postural control. Because the acquisition or learning of a motor skill relies in part on proprioception, numerous interventions were called proprioceptive training even if they did not directly isolate the contribution of improved proprioceptive acuity to postural control. To overcome such an issue, Aman et al. (2015) suggested defining proprioceptive training as an intervention that specifically targets proprioception and evaluates improvement in proprioception and its impact on sensorimotor function. In this context, using a robotic exoskeleton coupled with a virtual visual environment to induce proprioceptive-motor learning, Elangovan et al. (2018) reported improvement in wrist position sense acuity and spatial movement accuracy in an untrained, discrete wrist-pointing task in healthy older adults and parkinsonian patients. More related to postural control, a creative dance program that emphasized body awareness and postural control improved knee joint position sense, knee kinesthesia, and arm positioning in older adults (Marmeleira et al. 2009). Interestingly, a recent meta-analysis points toward positive effects of long-term Tai Chi practice on ankle proprioception in older adults (Zou et al. 2019), while Tai Chi has been largely documented to improve balance (Chen et al. 2012). Other interventions using exergames, which combine physical exercise and gaming (active video game exercises), have shown improvements in balance, mobility and strength (van Diest et al. 2013) along with proprioception in older adults (Sadeghi et al. 2017). One advantage of exergames is that they may be more enjoyable and thereby should increase adherence to the intervention (Valenzuela et al. 2018). It has been hypothesized that these types of interventions positively influence proprioception through enhancements of the sensitivity of proprioceptive sensors and cortical reorganization, which improves central processing of proprioceptive information (Han et al. 2015).

Another approach consists of augmented sensory feedback, defined here as the addition of sensory cues via auditory, tactile, or visual modalities to provide relevant information about posture. In 1978, Wannstedt and Herman were among the first to show that augmented sensory feedback could improve postural control in hemiparesis patients (Wannstedt and Herman 1978). Since then, various methods have been used, showing some relevant effects on proprioception and postural control (Sienko et al. 2018). For example, healthy older adults who trained with augmented sensory feedback showed a greater increase in reliance on vestibular inputs after training than a group who performed balance exercises alone (Bao et al. 2018). In this study, participants performed balance exercises with or without vibrotactile sensory augmentation. These results suggest that augmented sensory feedback can be used as a rehabilitation tool targeting postural control by reweighting the sensory sources to control upright standing. However, another study showed no overall benefit of balance training in healthy older adults when training was performed both with and without multimodal (vibrotactile, auditory, and visual) augmented sensory feedback (Lim et al. 2016).

An interesting emerging approach of augmented sensory feedback rests on applying mechanical or electrical noise over the limbs (Ribot-Ciscar et al. 2013). This approach relies on a stochastic resonance phenomenon (Chow et al. 1998) that is assumed to enhance detection, transmission, and discrimination of sensory signals by the addition of mechanical or electrical noise (McDonnell and Abbott 2009; Moss et al. 2004). For example, Ribot-Ciscar et al. (2013) indicated that ankle movement sense could be improved by adding an optimal level of mechanical noise to ankle muscle-tendon unit through random vibration. The authors suggested that mechanical noise may constitute a means of improving postural stability in subjects with sensory deficits. Similarly, electrical noise stimulation applied over the knee or leg muscles improved proprioception and postural control in young and older adults (Gravelle et al. 2002; Toledo et al. 2017).

If these different approaches to limiting the impact of aging on leg proprioception and its effects on postural control are very promising, the heterogeneity of the interventions, subject characteristics, and outcome measures does not allow for definitive conclusions on their usefulness and specific recommendations. Future studies are therefore warranted to optimize such interventions in the context of postural control in older adults.

CONCLUSIONS

Our objective was to provide a comprehensive review of how advancing age changes leg proprioception and to put forward evidence that these changes modify the control of upright standing in older adults. Several lines of evidence indicate that leg proprioception is altered with aging, and these alterations encompass among other mechanisms decreases in the sensitivity, acuity, and integration of the proprioceptive signal. The impact of these alterations on postural control is reflected in greater body sway and excursions of the center of pressure, a decrease in the relevance of the proprioceptive signal for postural control, an increase in antagonist coactivation, a greater reliance on visual information, and less automatic control of upright standing. These changes contribute to decrease the flexibility of the postural system and would ultimately impair postural control and the adaptative capacity to face changes in internal or external conditions (Fig. 3). Nonetheless, specific interventions may improve postural performance through changes induced with the proprioceptive system, although future work should document the characteristics of such interventions.

GRANTS

M. Henry is supported by a PhD grant from the Fonds National pour la Recherche Scientifique (FNRS).

DISCLOSURES

No conflicts of interest, financial or otherwise, are declared by the authors.

AUTHOR CONTRIBUTIONS

M.H. and S.B. prepared figures; M.H. and S.B. drafted manuscript; M.H. and S.B. edited and revised manuscript; M.H. and S.B. approved final version of manuscript.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS

The authors thank Prof. Roger Enoka (University of Colorado Boulder) and Prof. Stephan Swinnen (KU Leuven) for providing comments on draft versions of the manuscript and Rowan Smart (University of British Columbia) for invaluable editing work.

REFERENCES

  • Abe O, Aoki S, Hayashi N, Yamada H, Kunimatsu A, Mori H, Yoshikawa T, Okubo T, Ohtomo K. Normal aging in the central nervous system: quantitative MR diffusion-tensor analysis. Neurobiol Aging 23: 433–441, 2002. doi: 10.1016/S0197-4580(01)00318-9. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Abrahamová D, Hlavačka F. Age-related changes of human balance during quiet stance. Physiol Res 57: 957–964, 2008. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Abrahámová D, Mancini M, Hlavacka F, Chiari L. The age-related changes of trunk responses to Achilles tendon vibration. Neurosci Lett 467: 220–224, 2009. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2009.10.041. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Adamo DE, Alexander NB, Brown SH. The influence of age and physical activity on upper limb proprioceptive ability. J Aging Phys Act 17: 272–293, 2009. doi: 10.1123/japa.17.3.272. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Aman JE, Elangovan N, Yeh IL, Konczak J. The effectiveness of proprioceptive training for improving motor function: a systematic review. Front Hum Neurosci 8: 1075, 2015. doi: 10.3389/fnhum.2014.01075. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Aniss AM, Diener HC, Hore J, Gandevia SC, Burke D. Behavior of human muscle receptors when reliant on proprioceptive feedback during standing. J Neurophysiol 64: 661–670, 1990. doi: 10.1152/jn.1990.64.2.661. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ansems GE, Allen TJ, Proske U. Position sense at the human forearm in the horizontal plane during loading and vibration of elbow muscles. J Physiol 576: 445–455, 2006. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2006.115097. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Anson E, Bigelow RT, Swenor B, Deshpande N, Studenski S, Jeka JJ, Agrawal Y. Loss of peripheral sensory function explains much of the increase in postural sway in healthy older adults. Front Aging Neurosci 9: 202, 2017. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2017.00202. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Bao T, Carender WJ, Kinnaird C, Barone VJ, Peethambaran G, Whitney SL, Kabeto M, Seidler RD, Sienko KH. Effects of long-term balance training with vibrotactile sensory augmentation among community-dwelling healthy older adults: a randomized preliminary study. J Neuroeng Rehabil 15: 5, 2018. doi: 10.1186/s12984-017-0339-6. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Baudry S. Aging changes the contribution of spinal and corticospinal pathways to control balance. Exerc Sport Sci Rev 44: 104–109, 2016. doi: 10.1249/JES.0000000000000080. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Baudry S, Collignon S, Duchateau J. Influence of age and posture on spinal and corticospinal excitability. Exp Gerontol 69: 62–69, 2015. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2015.06.006. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Baudry S, Duchateau J. Age-related influence of vision and proprioception on Ia presynaptic inhibition in soleus muscle during upright stance. J Physiol 590: 5541–5554, 2012. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2012.228932. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Baudry S, Gaillard V. Cognitive demand does not influence the responsiveness of homonymous Ia afferents pathway during postural dual task in young and elderly adults. Eur J Appl Physiol 114: 295–303, 2014. doi: 10.1007/s00421-013-2775-8. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Baudry S, Lecoeuvre G, Duchateau J. Age-related changes in the behavior of the muscle-tendon unit of the gastrocnemius medialis during upright stance. J Appl Physiol (1985) 112: 296–304, 2012. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00913.2011. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Baudry S, Maerz AH, Enoka RM. Presynaptic modulation of Ia afferents in young and old adults when performing force and position control. J Neurophysiol 103: 623–631, 2010. doi: 10.1152/jn.00839.2009. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Baudry S, Penzer F, Duchateau J. Input-output characteristics of soleus homonymous Ia afferents and corticospinal pathways during upright standing differ between young and elderly adults. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 210: 667–677, 2014a. doi: 10.1111/apha.12233. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Baudry S, Penzer F, Duchateau J. Vision and proprioception do not influence the excitability of the corticomotoneuronal pathway during upright standing in young and elderly adults. Neuroscience 268: 247–254, 2014b. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2014.03.026. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Benjuya N, Melzer I, Kaplanski J. Aging-induced shifts from a reliance on sensory input to muscle cocontraction during balanced standing. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 59: M166–M171, 2004. doi: 10.1093/gerona/59.2.M166. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Berard J, Fung J, Lamontagne A. Impact of aging on visual reweighting during locomotion. Clin Neurophysiol 123: 1422–1428, 2012. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2011.11.081. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Berger L, Bernard-Demanze L. Age-related effects of a memorizing spatial task in the adults and elderly postural control. Gait Posture 33: 300–302, 2011. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2010.10.082. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Bergin PS, Bronstein AM, Murray NM, Sancovic S, Zeppenfeld DK. Body sway and vibration perception thresholds in normal aging and in patients with polyneuropathy. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 58: 335–340, 1995. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.58.3.335. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Bessou P, Emonet-Dénand F, Laporte Y. Motor fibres innervating extrafusal and intrafusal muscle fibres in the cat. J Physiol 180: 649–672, 1965. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1965.sp007722. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Billot M, Simoneau EM, Van Hoecke J, Martin A. Age-related relative increases in electromyography activity and torque according to the maximal capacity during upright standing. Eur J Appl Physiol 109: 669–680, 2010. doi: 10.1007/s00421-010-1397-7. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Blum KP, Lamotte d’Incamps B, Zytnicki D, Ting LH. Force encoding in muscle spindles during stretch of passive muscle. PLoS Comput Biol 13: e1005767, 2017. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005767. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Boisgontier MP, Cheval B, Chalavi S, van Ruitenbeek P, Leunissen I, Levin O, Nieuwboer A, Swinnen SP. Individual differences in brainstem and basal ganglia structure predict postural control and balance loss in young and older adults. Neurobiol Aging 50: 47–59, 2017. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2016.10.024. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Boisgontier MP, Nougier V. Ageing of internal models: from a continuous to an intermittent proprioceptive control of movement. Age (Dordr) 35: 1339–1355, 2013. doi: 10.1007/s11357-012-9436-4. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Boisgontier MP, Van Halewyck F, Corporaal SH, Willacker L, Van Den Bergh V, Beets IA, Levin O, Swinnen SP. Vision of the active limb impairs bimanual motor tracking in young and older adults. Front Aging Neurosci 6: 320, 2014. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2014.00320. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Borger LL, Whitney SL, Redfern MS, Furman JM. The influence of dynamic visual environments on postural sway in the elderly. J Vestib Res 9: 197–205, 1999. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Boxer PA, Morales FR, Chase MH. Alterations of group Ia-motoneuron monosynaptic EPSPs in aged cats. Exp Neurol 100: 583–595, 1988. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(88)90042-8. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Brumagne S, Cordo P, Verschueren S. Proprioceptive weighting changes in persons with low back pain and elderly persons during upright standing. Neurosci Lett 366: 63–66, 2004. doi: 10.1016/j.neulet.2004.05.013. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Bugnariu N, Fung J. Aging and selective sensorimotor strategies in the regulation of upright balance. J Neuroeng Rehabil 4: 19, 2007. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-4-19. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Burgess PR, Wei JY, Clark FJ, Simon J. Signaling of kinesthetic information by peripheral sensory receptors. Annu Rev Neurosci 5: 171–188, 1982. doi: 10.1146/annurev.ne.05.030182.001131. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Burke D, Hagbarth KE, Löfstedt L, Wallin BG. The responses of human muscle spindle endings to vibration of non-contracting muscles. J Physiol 261: 673–693, 1976. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1976.sp011580. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Burke JR, Schutten MC, Koceja DM, Kamen G. Age-dependent effects of muscle vibration and the Jendrassik maneuver on the patellar tendon reflex response. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 77: 600–604, 1996. doi: 10.1016/S0003-9993(96)90302-0. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Butchart P, Farquhar R, Part NJ, Roberts RC. The effect of age and voluntary contraction on presynaptic inhibition of soleus muscle Ia afferent terminals in man. Exp Physiol 78: 235–242, 1993. doi: 10.1113/expphysiol.1993.sp003683. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Butler AA, Lord SR, Rogers MW, Fitzpatrick RC. Muscle weakness impairs the proprioceptive control of human standing. Brain Res 1242: 244–251, 2008. doi: 10.1016/j.brainres.2008.03.094. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Carpenter MG, Murnaghan CD, Inglis JT. Shifting the balance: evidence of an exploratory role for postural sway. Neuroscience 171: 196–204, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2010.08.030. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Chalmers GR, Knutzen KM. Soleus Hoffmann-reflex modulation during walking in healthy elderly and young adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 55: B570–B579, 2000. doi: 10.1093/gerona/55.12.B570. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Chase MH, Morales FR, Boxer PA, Fung SJ. Aging of motoneurons and synaptic processes in the cat. Exp Neurol 90: 471–478, 1985. doi: 10.1016/0014-4886(85)90035-4. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Chen EW, Fu AS, Chan KM, Tsang WW. The effects of Tai Chi on the balance control of elderly persons with visual impairment: a randomised clinical trial. Age Ageing 41: 254–259, 2012. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afr146. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Chow CC, Imhoff TT, Collins JJ. Enhancing aperiodic stochastic resonance through noise modulation. Chaos 8: 616–620, 1998. doi: 10.1063/1.166343. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Chung SG, Van Rey EM, Bai Z, Rogers MW, Roth EJ, Zhang LQ. Aging-related neuromuscular changes characterized by tendon reflex system properties. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 86: 318–327, 2005. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2004.04.048. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Cremer R, Zeef EJ. What kind of noise increases with age? J Gerontol 42: 515–518, 1987. doi: 10.1093/geronj/42.5.515. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Day J, Bent LR, Birznieks I, Macefield VG, Cresswell AG. Muscle spindles in human tibialis anterior encode muscle fascicle length changes. J Neurophysiol 117: 1489–1498, 2017. doi: 10.1152/jn.00374.2016. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Day JT, Lichtwark GA, Cresswell AG. Tibialis anterior muscle fascicle dynamics adequately represent postural sway during standing balance. J Appl Physiol (1985) 115: 1742–1750, 2013. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00517.2013. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Deshpande N, Simonsick E, Metter EJ, Ko S, Ferrucci L, Studenski S. Ankle proprioceptive acuity is associated with objective as well as self-report measures of balance, mobility, and physical function. Age (Dordr) 38: 53, 2016. doi: 10.1007/s11357-016-9918-x. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Di Giulio I, Maganaris CN, Baltzopoulos V, Loram ID. The proprioceptive and agonist roles of gastrocnemius, soleus and tibialis anterior muscles in maintaining human upright posture. J Physiol 587: 2399–2416, 2009. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2009.168690. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Donath L, Kurz E, Roth R, Zahner L, Faude O. Different ankle muscle coordination patterns and co-activation during quiet stance between young adults and seniors do not change after a bout of high intensity training. BMC Geriatr 15: 19, 2015. doi: 10.1186/s12877-015-0017-0. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Doumas M, Smolders C, Krampe RT. Task prioritization in aging: effects of sensory information on concurrent posture and memory performance. Exp Brain Res 187: 275–281, 2008. doi: 10.1007/s00221-008-1302-3. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Earles DR, Koceja DM, Shively CW. Environmental changes in soleus H-reflex excitability in young and elderly subjects. Int J Neurosci 105: 1–13, 2000. doi: 10.3109/00207450009003261. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ehsani H, Mohler J, Marlinski V, Rashedi E, Toosizadeh N. The influence of mechanical vibration on local and central balance control. J Biomech 71: 59–66, 2018. doi: 10.1016/j.jbiomech.2018.01.027. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Eikema DJ, Hatzitaki V, Tzovaras D, Papaxanthis C. Age-dependent modulation of sensory reweighting for controlling posture in a dynamic virtual environment. Age (Dordr) 34: 1381–1392, 2012. doi: 10.1007/s11357-011-9310-9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Eklund G. General features of vibration-induced effects on balance. Ups J Med Sci 77: 112–124, 1972. doi: 10.1517/03009734000000016. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Elangovan N, Tuite PJ, Konczak J. Somatosensory training improves proprioception and untrained motor function in Parkinson’s disease. Front Neurol 9: 1053, 2018. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.01053. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Eysel-Gosepath K, McCrum C, Epro G, Brüggemann GP, Karamanidis K. Visual and proprioceptive contributions to postural control of upright stance in unilateral vestibulopathy. Somatosens Mot Res 33: 72–78, 2016. doi: 10.1080/08990220.2016.1178635. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fetz EE, Gustafsson B. Relation between shapes of post-synaptic potentials and changes in firing probability of cat motoneurones. J Physiol 341: 387–410, 1983. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1983.sp014812. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Finley JM, Dhaher YY, Perreault EJ. Contributions of feed-forward and feedback strategies at the human ankle during control of unstable loads. Exp Brain Res 217: 53–66, 2012. doi: 10.1007/s00221-011-2972-9. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fitzpatrick R, McCloskey DI. Proprioceptive, visual and vestibular thresholds for the perception of sway during standing in humans. J Physiol 478: 173–186, 1994. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1994.sp020240. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Fitzpatrick R, Rogers DK, McCloskey DI. Stable human standing with lower-limb muscle afferents providing the only sensory input. J Physiol 480: 395–403, 1994. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1994.sp020369. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Franz JR, Francis CA, Allen MS, O’Connor SM, Thelen DG. Advanced age brings a greater reliance on visual feedback to maintain balance during walking. Hum Mov Sci 40: 381–392, 2015. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2015.01.012. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Frontera WR, Hughes VA, Lutz KJ, Evans WJ. A cross-sectional study of muscle strength and mass in 45- to 78-yr-old men and women. J Appl Physiol (1985) 71: 644–650, 1991. doi: 10.1152/jappl.1991.71.2.644. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gaillardin F, Baudry S. Influence of working memory and executive function on stair ascent and descent in young and older adults. Exp Gerontol 106: 74–79, 2018. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2018.02.022. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Goble DJ, Coxon JP, Van Impe A, Geurts M, Doumas M, Wenderoth N, Swinnen SP. Brain activity during ankle proprioceptive stimulation predicts balance performance in young and older adults. J Neurosci 31: 16344–16352, 2011. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.4159-11.2011. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Goble DJ, Coxon JP, Van Impe A, Geurts M, Van Hecke W, Sunaert S, Wenderoth N, Swinnen SP. The neural basis of central proprioceptive processing in older versus younger adults: an important sensory role for right putamen. Hum Brain Mapp 33: 895–908, 2012. doi: 10.1002/hbm.21257. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Good CD, Johnsrude IS, Ashburner J, Henson RN, Friston KJ, Frackowiak RS. A voxel-based morphometric study of ageing in 465 normal adult human brains. Neuroimage 14: 21–36, 2001. doi: 10.1006/nimg.2001.0786. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Goodwin GM, McCloskey DI, Matthews PB. Proprioceptive illusions induced by muscle vibration: contribution by muscle spindles to perception? Science 175: 1382–1384, 1972. doi: 10.1126/science.175.4028.1382. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gottfries CG. Neurochemical aspects on aging and diseases with cognitive impairment. J Neurosci Res 27: 541–547, 1990. doi: 10.1002/jnr.490270415. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Gravelle DC, Laughton CA, Dhruv NT, Katdare KD, Niemi JB, Lipsitz LA, Collins JJ. Noise-enhanced balance control in older adults. Neuroreport 13: 1853–1856, 2002. doi: 10.1097/00001756-200210280-00004. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Haibach P, Slobounov S, Newell K. Egomotion and vection in young and elderly adults. Gerontology 55: 637–643, 2009. doi: 10.1159/000235816. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Han J, Anson J, Waddington G, Adams R, Liu Y. The role of ankle proprioception for balance control in relation to sports performance and injury. BioMed Res Int 2015: 842804, 2015. doi: 10.1155/2015/842804. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hay L, Bard C, Fleury M, Teasdale N. Availability of visual and proprioceptive afferent messages and postural control in elderly adults. Exp Brain Res 108: 129–139, 1996. doi: 10.1007/BF00242910. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Heuninckx S, Debaere F, Wenderoth N, Verschueren S, Swinnen SP. Ipsilateral coordination deficits and central processing requirements associated with coordination as a function of aging. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 59: P225–P232, 2004. doi: 10.1093/geronb/59.5.P225. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Heuninckx S, Wenderoth N, Debaere F, Peeters R, Swinnen SP. Neural basis of aging: the penetration of cognition into action control. J Neurosci 25: 6787–6796, 2005. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.1263-05.2005. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Heuninckx S, Wenderoth N, Swinnen SP. Systems neuroplasticity in the aging brain: recruiting additional neural resources for successful motor performance in elderly persons. J Neurosci 28: 91–99, 2008. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.3300-07.2008. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hoch MC, Russell DM. Plantar cooling does not affect standing balance: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Gait Posture 43: 1–8, 2016. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2015.10.011. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hogan N. Adaptive control of mechanical impedance by coactivation of antagonist muscles. IEEE Trans Automat Contr 29: 681–690, 1984. doi: 10.1109/TAC.1984.1103644. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Honeycutt CF, Gottschall JS, Nichols TR. Electromyographic responses from the hindlimb muscles of the decerebrate cat to horizontal support surface perturbations. J Neurophysiol 101: 2751–2761, 2009. doi: 10.1152/jn.91040.2008. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hong SL, Rebec GV. A new perspective on behavioral inconsistency and neural noise in aging: compensatory speeding of neural communication. Front Aging Neurosci 4: 27, 2012. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2012.00027. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Horak FB. Postural orientation and equilibrium: what do we need to know about neural control of balance to prevent falls? Age Ageing 35, Suppl 2: ii7–ii11, 2006. doi: 10.1093/ageing/afl077. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Horak FB, Shupert CL, Mirka A. Components of postural dyscontrol in the elderly: a review. Neurobiol Aging 10: 727–738, 1989. doi: 10.1016/0197-4580(89)90010-9. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Hunter SK, Pereira HM, Keenan KG. The aging neuromuscular system and motor performance. J Appl Physiol (1985) 121: 982–995, 2016. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00475.2016. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Inglis JT, Horak FB, Shupert CL, Jones-Rycewicz C. The importance of somatosensory information in triggering and scaling automatic postural responses in humans. Exp Brain Res 101: 159–164, 1994. doi: 10.1007/BF00243226. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Inglis JT, Macpherson JM. Bilateral labyrinthectomy in the cat: effects on the postural response to translation. J Neurophysiol 73: 1181–1191, 1995. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.3.1181. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ingram HA, van Donkelaar P, Cole J, Vercher JL, Gauthier GM, Miall RC. The role of proprioception and attention in a visuomotor adaptation task. Exp Brain Res 132: 114–126, 2000. doi: 10.1007/s002219900322. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ito T, Sakai Y, Yamazaki K, Nishio R, Ito Y, Morita Y. Postural strategy in elderly, middle-aged, and young people during local vibratory stimulation for proprioceptive inputs. Geriatrics (Basel) 3: 93, 2018. doi: 10.3390/geriatrics3040093. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Jacobs JV, Horak FB. Cortical control of postural responses. J Neural Transm (Vienna) 114: 1339–1348, 2007. doi: 10.1007/s00702-007-0657-0. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Jeka JJ, Allison LK, Kiemel T. The dynamics of visual reweighting in healthy and fall-prone older adults. J Mot Behav 42: 197–208, 2010. doi: 10.1080/00222895.2010.481693. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kabbaligere R, Lee BC, Layne CS. Balancing sensory inputs: sensory reweighting of ankle proprioception and vision during a bipedal posture task. Gait Posture 52: 244–250, 2017. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.12.009. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kail R. The neural noise hypothesis: evidence from processing speed in adults with multiple sclerosis. Aging Neuropsychol Cogn 4: 157–165, 1997. doi: 10.1080/13825589708256644. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kalpouzos G, Chételat G, Baron JC, Landeau B, Mevel K, Godeau C, Barré L, Constans JM, Viader F, Eustache F, Desgranges B. Voxel-based mapping of brain gray matter volume and glucose metabolism profiles in normal aging. Neurobiol Aging 30: 112–124, 2009. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2007.05.019. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kararizou E, Manta P, Kalfakis N, Vassilopoulos D. Morphometric study of the human muscle spindle. Anal Quant Cytol Histol 27: 1–4, 2005. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Kavounoudias A, Gilhodes JC, Roll R, Roll JP. From balance regulation to body orientation: two goals for muscle proprioceptive information processing? Exp Brain Res 124: 80–88, 1999. doi: 10.1007/s002210050602. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kilgour AH, Todd OM, Starr JM. A systematic review of the evidence that brain structure is related to muscle structure and their relationship to brain and muscle function in humans over the lifecourse. BMC Geriatr 14: 85, 2014. doi: 10.1186/1471-2318-14-85. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Kim GH, Suzuki S, Kanda K. Age-related physiological and morphological changes of muscle spindles in rats. J Physiol 582: 525–538, 2007. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2007.130120. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Klass M, Baudry S, Duchateau J. Modulation of reflex responses in activated ankle dorsiflexors differs in healthy young and elderly subjects. Eur J Appl Physiol 111: 1909–1916, 2011. doi: 10.1007/s00421-010-1815-x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Knellwolf TP, Burton AR, Hammam E, Macefield VG. Firing properties of muscle spindles supplying the intrinsic foot muscles of humans in unloaded and freestanding conditions. J Neurophysiol 121: 74–84, 2019. doi: 10.1152/jn.00539.2018. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Koceja DM, Trimble MH, Earles DR. Inhibition of the soleus H-reflex in standing man. Brain Res 629: 155–158, 1993. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(93)90495-9. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lara J, Godfrey A, Evans E, Heaven B, Brown LJ, Barron E, Rochester L, Meyer TD, Mathers JC. Towards measurement of the Healthy Ageing Phenotype in lifestyle-based intervention studies. Maturitas 76: 189–199, 2013. doi: 10.1016/j.maturitas.2013.07.007. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Latash ML. Muscle coactivation: definitions, mechanisms, and functions. J Neurophysiol 120: 88–104, 2018. doi: 10.1152/jn.00084.2018. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Laughton CA, Slavin M, Katdare K, Nolan L, Bean JF, Kerrigan DC, Phillips E, Lipsitz LA, Collins JJ. Aging, muscle activity, and balance control: physiologic changes associated with balance impairment. Gait Posture 18: 101–108, 2003. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(02)00200-X. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Le Goic M, Wang D, Vidal C, Chiarovano E, Lecompte J, Laporte S, Duysens J, Vidal PP. An initial passive phase that limits the time to recover and emphasizes the role of proprioceptive information. Front Neurol 9: 986, 2018. [Erratum in Front Neurol 10: 118, 2019.] doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00986. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lephart SM, Pincivero DM, Giraido JL, Fu FH. The role of proprioception in the management and rehabilitation of athletic injuries. Am J Sports Med 25: 130–137, 1997. doi: 10.1177/036354659702500126. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lim SB, Horslen BC, Davis JR, Allum JH, Carpenter MG. Benefits of multi-session balance and gait training with multi-modal biofeedback in healthy older adults. Gait Posture 47: 10–17, 2016. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2016.03.017. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Liu JX, Eriksson PO, Thornell LE, Pedrosa-Domellöf F. Fiber content and myosin heavy chain composition of muscle spindles in aged human biceps brachii. J Histochem Cytochem 53: 445–454, 2005. doi: 10.1369/jhc.4A6257.2005. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Loram ID, Lakie M. Direct measurement of human ankle stiffness during quiet standing: the intrinsic mechanical stiffness is insufficient for stability. J Physiol 545: 1041–1053, 2002. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.025049. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Loram ID, Maganaris CN, Lakie M. Paradoxical muscle movement in human standing. J Physiol 556: 683–689, 2004. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2004.062398. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lord SR, Clark RD, Webster IW. Postural stability and associated physiological factors in a population of aged persons. J Gerontol 46: M69–M76, 1991. doi: 10.1093/geronj/46.3.M69. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Lord SR, Ward JA. Age-associated differences in sensori-motor function and balance in community dwelling women. Age Ageing 23: 452–460, 1994. doi: 10.1093/ageing/23.6.452. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Macefield G, Gandevia SC, Burke D. Perceptual responses to microstimulation of single afferents innervating joints, muscles and skin of the human hand. J Physiol 429: 113–129, 1990. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.1990.sp018247. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Macefield VG. Physiological characteristics of low-threshold mechanoreceptors in joints, muscle and skin in human subjects. Clin Exp Pharmacol Physiol 32: 135–144, 2005. doi: 10.1111/j.1440-1681.2005.04143.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Maitre J, Jully JL, Gasnier Y, Paillard T. Chronic physical activity preserves efficiency of proprioception in postural control in older women. J Rehabil Res Dev 50: 811–820, 2013. doi: 10.1682/JRRD.2012.08.0141. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Maki BE, Holliday PJ, Topper AK. A prospective study of postural balance and risk of falling in an ambulatory and independent elderly population. J Gerontol 49: M72–M84, 1994. doi: 10.1093/geronj/49.2.M72. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Manchester D, Woollacott M, Zederbauer-Hylton N, Marin O. Visual, vestibular and somatosensory contributions to balance control in the older adult. J Gerontol 44: M118–M127, 1989. doi: 10.1093/geronj/44.4.M118. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Marmeleira JF, Pereira C, Cruz-Ferreira A, Fretes V, Pisco R, Fernandes OM. Creative dance can enhance proprioception in older adults. J Sports Med Phys Fitness 49: 480–485, 2009. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Martin JH, Jessel TM. Modality coding in the somatic sensory systems. In: Principles of Neural Science (3rd ed.), edited by Kandel R, Schwartz JH, Jessel TM. New York: Elsevier, 1991, p. 341–353. [Google Scholar]
  • Matthews PB. Muscle spindles: their messages and their fusimotor supply. In: Handbook of Physiology. The Nervous System, edited by Brookhart JM, Mountcastle WB, Brooks VB. Bethesda, MD: American Physiological Society, 1981, p. 189–228. doi: 10.1002/cphy.cp010206. [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Mauritz KH, Dietz V. Characteristics of postural instability induced by ischemic blocking of leg afferents. Exp Brain Res 38: 117–119, 1980. doi: 10.1007/BF00237939. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • McCloskey DI. Differences between the senses of movement and position shown by the effects of loading and vibration of muscles in man. Brain Res 61: 119–131, 1973. doi: 10.1016/0006-8993(73)90521-0. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • McCloskey DI. Kinesthetic sensibility. Physiol Rev 58: 763–820, 1978. doi: 10.1152/physrev.1978.58.4.763. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • McDonnell MD, Abbott D. What is stochastic resonance? Definitions, misconceptions, debates, and its relevance to biology. PLOS Comput Biol 5: e1000348, 2009. doi: 10.1371/journal.pcbi.1000348. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Melzer I, Benjuya N, Kaplanski J. Age-related changes of postural control: effect of cognitive tasks. Gerontology 47: 189–194, 2001. doi: 10.1159/000052797. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Miwa T, Miwa Y, Kanda K. Dynamic and static sensitivities of muscle spindle primary endings in aged rats to ramp stretch. Neurosci Lett 201: 179–182, 1995. doi: 10.1016/0304-3940(95)12165-X. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Morales FR, Boxer PA, Fung SJ, Chase MH. Basic electrophysiological properties of spinal cord motoneurons during old age in the cat. J Neurophysiol 58: 180–194, 1987. doi: 10.1152/jn.1987.58.1.180. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Morita H, Shindo M, Yanagawa S, Yoshida T, Momoi H, Yanagisawa N. Progressive decrease in heteronymous monosynaptic Ia facilitation with human ageing. Exp Brain Res 104: 167–170, 1995. doi: 10.1007/BF00229867. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Moss F, Ward LM, Sannita WG. Stochastic resonance and sensory information processing: a tutorial and review of application. Clin Neurophysiol 115: 267–281, 2004. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.09.014. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Mouthon A, Ruffieux J, Mouthon M, Hoogewoud HM, Annoni JM, Taube W. Age-related differences in cortical and subcortical activities during observation and motor imagery of dynamic postural tasks: an fMRI study. Neural Plast 2018: 1598178, 2018. doi: 10.1155/2018/1598178. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Mozolic JL, Hugenschmidt CE, Peiffer AM, Laurienti PJ. Multisensory integration and aging. In: The Neural Bases of Multisensory Processes, Frontiers in Neuroscience, edited by Murray MM, Wallace MT. Boca Raton, FL: CRC/Taylor & Francis, 2012. [Google Scholar]
  • Nagai K, Yamada M, Uemura K, Yamada Y, Ichihashi N, Tsuboyama T. Differences in muscle coactivation during postural control between healthy older and young adults. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 53: 338–343, 2011. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2011.01.003. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Nakagawa H, Ohashi N, Watanabe Y, Mizukoshi K. The contribution of proprioception to posture control in normal subjects. Acta Otolaryngol Suppl 113, Suppl 504: 112–116, 1993. doi: 10.3109/00016489309128134. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • O’Connor KW, Loughlin PJ, Redfern MS, Sparto PJ. Postural adaptations to repeated optic flow stimulation in older adults. Gait Posture 28: 385–391, 2008. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2008.01.010. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ortega JD, Farley CT. Effects of aging on mechanical efficiency and muscle activation during level and uphill walking. J Electromyogr Kinesiol 25: 193–198, 2015. doi: 10.1016/j.jelekin.2014.09.003. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Orth M, Rothwell JC. The cortical silent period: intrinsic variability and relation to the waveform of the transcranial magnetic stimulation pulse. Clin Neurophysiol 115: 1076–1082, 2004. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2003.12.025. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ozdemir RA, Contreras-Vidal JL, Paloski WH. Cortical control of upright stance in elderly. Mech Ageing Dev 169: 19–31, 2018. doi: 10.1016/j.mad.2017.12.004. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Papegaaij S, Taube W, van Keeken HG, Otten E, Baudry S, Hortobágyi T. Postural challenge affects motor cortical activity in young and old adults. Exp Gerontol 73: 78–85, 2016. doi: 10.1016/j.exger.2015.11.015. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Pavailler S, Hintzy F, Horvais N, Forestier N. Cutaneous stimulation at the ankle: a differential effect on proprioceptive postural control according to the participants’ preferred sensory strategy. J Foot Ankle Res 9: 9, 2016. doi: 10.1186/s13047-016-0140-y. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Penzer F, Duchateau J, Baudry S.. Contribution of visual and proprioceptive information in postural control differs with age. Annual Congress of the European College of Sport Science, Malmö, Sweden, June 24–27 2015. [Google Scholar]
  • Peterka RJ. Sensorimotor integration in human postural control. J Neurophysiol 88: 1097–1118, 2002. doi: 10.1152/jn.2002.88.3.1097. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Peters RM, Dalton BH, Blouin JS, Inglis JT. Precise coding of ankle angle and velocity by human calf muscle spindles. Neuroscience 349: 98–105, 2017. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.02.034. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Petersen NT, Pyndt HS, Nielsen JB. Investigating human motor control by transcranial magnetic stimulation. Exp Brain Res 152: 1–16, 2003. doi: 10.1007/s00221-003-1537-y. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Petrella M, Gramani-Say K, Serrão PR, Lessi GC, Barela JA, Carvalho RP, Mattiello SM. Measuring postural control during mini-squat posture in men with early knee osteoarthritis. Hum Mov Sci 52: 108–116, 2017. doi: 10.1016/j.humov.2017.01.011. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Petrella RJ, Lattanzio PJ, Nelson MG. Effect of age and activity on knee joint proprioception. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 76: 235–241, 1997. doi: 10.1097/00002060-199705000-00015. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Piitulainen H, Seipäjärvi S, Avela J, Parviainen T, Walker S. Cortical proprioceptive processing is altered by aging. Front Aging Neurosci 10: 147, 2018. doi: 10.3389/fnagi.2018.00147. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Prochazka A. Sensorimotor gain control: a basic strategy of motor systems? Prog Neurobiol 33: 281–307, 1989. doi: 10.1016/0301-0082(89)90004-X. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Proske U, Allen T. The neural basis of the senses of effort, force and heaviness. Exp Brain Res 237: 589–599, 2019. doi: 10.1007/s00221-018-5460-7. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Proske U, Gandevia SC. The proprioceptive senses: their roles in signaling body shape, body position and movement, and muscle force. Physiol Rev 92: 1651–1697, 2012. doi: 10.1152/physrev.00048.2011. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Proske U, Gregory JE. Signalling properties of muscle spindles and tendon organs. Adv Exp Med Biol 508: 5–12, 2002. doi: 10.1007/978-1-4615-0713-0_1. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Proske U, Wise AK, Gregory JE. The role of muscle receptors in the detection of movements. Prog Neurobiol 60: 85–96, 2000. doi: 10.1016/S0301-0082(99)00022-2. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Pyykkö I, Jäntti P, Aalto H. Postural control in elderly subjects. Age Ageing 19: 215–221, 1990. doi: 10.1093/ageing/19.3.215. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Quoniam C, Hay L, Roll JP, Harlay F. Age effects on reflex and postural responses to propriomuscular inputs generated by tendon vibration. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 50A: B155–B165, 1995. doi: 10.1093/gerona/50A.3.B155. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Rajachandrakumar R, Mann J, Schinkel-Ivy A, Mansfield A. Exploring the relationship between stability and variability of the centre of mass and centre of pressure. Gait Posture 63: 254–259, 2018. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2018.05.008. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Rankin JK, Woollacott MH, Shumway-Cook A, Brown LA. Cognitive influence on postural stability: a neuromuscular analysis in young and older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 55: M112–M119, 2000. doi: 10.1093/gerona/55.3.M112. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Reuter-Lorenz PA, Lustig C. Brain aging: reorganizing discoveries about the aging mind. Curr Opin Neurobiol 15: 245–251, 2005. doi: 10.1016/j.conb.2005.03.016. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ribeiro F, Oliveira J. Effect of physical exercise and age on knee joint position sense. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 51: 64–67, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2009.07.006. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Ribot-Ciscar E, Hospod V, Aimonetti JM. Noise-enhanced kinaesthesia: a psychophysical and microneurographic study. Exp Brain Res 228: 503–511, 2013. doi: 10.1007/s00221-013-3581-6. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Riemann BL, Lephart SM. The sensorimotor system, part I: the physiologic basis of functional joint stability. J Athl Train 37: 71–79, 2002. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [Google Scholar]
  • Roll JP, Vedel JP. Kinaesthetic role of muscle afferents in man, studied by tendon vibration and microneurography. Exp Brain Res 47: 177–190, 1982. doi: 10.1007/BF00239377. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Rosano C, Aizenstein HJ, Studenski S, Newman AB. A regions-of-interest volumetric analysis of mobility limitations in community-dwelling older adults. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 62: 1048–1055, 2007. doi: 10.1093/gerona/62.9.1048. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Sabbahi MA, Sedgwick EM. Age-related changes in monosynaptic reflex excitability. J Gerontol 37: 24–32, 1982. doi: 10.1093/geronj/37.1.24. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Sadeghi H, Hakim MN, Hamid TA, Amri SB, Razeghi M, Farazdaghi M, Shakoor E. The effect of exergaming on knee proprioception in older men: a randomized controlled trial. Arch Gerontol Geriatr 69: 144–150, 2017. doi: 10.1016/j.archger.2016.11.009. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Sainburg RL, Ghilardi MF, Poizner H, Ghez C. Control of limb dynamics in normal subjects and patients without proprioception. J Neurophysiol 73: 820–835, 1995. doi: 10.1152/jn.1995.73.2.820. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Salat DH, Buckner RL, Snyder AZ, Greve DN, Desikan RS, Busa E, Morris JC, Dale AM, Fischl B. Thinning of the cerebral cortex in aging. Cereb Cortex 14: 721–730, 2004. doi: 10.1093/cercor/bhh032. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Sarabon N, Rosker J, Loefler S, Kern H. The effect of vision elimination during quiet stance tasks with different feet positions. Gait Posture 38: 708–711, 2013. doi: 10.1016/j.gaitpost.2013.03.005. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Scaglioni G, Narici MV, Maffiuletti NA, Pensini M, Martin A. Effect of ageing on the electrical and mechanical properties of human soleus motor units activated by the H reflex and M wave. J Physiol 548: 649–661, 2003. doi: 10.1113/jphysiol.2002.032763. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Seidler RD, Bernard JA, Burutolu TB, Fling BW, Gordon MT, Gwin JT, Kwak Y, Lipps DB. Motor control and aging: links to age-related brain structural, functional, and biochemical effects. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 34: 721–733, 2010. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2009.10.005. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Shaffer SW, Harrison AL. Aging of the somatosensory system: a translational perspective. Phys Ther 87: 193–207, 2007. doi: 10.2522/ptj.20060083. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Sienko KH, Seidler RD, Carender WJ, Goodworth AD, Whitney SL, Peterka RJ. Potential mechanisms of sensory augmentation systems on human balance control. Front Neurol 9: 944, 2018. doi: 10.3389/fneur.2018.00944. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Simoneau M, Teasdale N, Bourdin C, Bard C, Fleury M, Nougier V. Aging and postural control: postural perturbations caused by changing the visual anchor. J Am Geriatr Soc 47: 235–240, 1999. doi: 10.1111/j.1532-5415.1999.tb04584.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Soto O, Valls-Solé J, Shanahan P, Rothwell J. Reduction of intracortical inhibition in soleus muscle during postural activity. J Neurophysiol 96: 1711–1717, 2006. doi: 10.1152/jn.00133.2006. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Speers RA, Kuo AD, Horak FB. Contributions of altered sensation and feedback responses to changes in coordination of postural control due to aging. Gait Posture 16: 20–30, 2002. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(02)00003-6. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Stapley PJ, Ting LH, Hulliger M, Macpherson JM. Automatic postural responses are delayed by pyridoxine-induced somatosensory loss. J Neurosci 22: 5803–5807, 2002. doi: 10.1523/JNEUROSCI.22-14-05803.2002. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Sullivan EV, Pfefferbaum A. Diffusion tensor imaging and aging. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 30: 749–761, 2006. doi: 10.1016/j.neubiorev.2006.06.002. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Sundermier L, Woollacott MH, Jensen JL, Moore S. Postural sensitivity to visual flow in aging adults with and without balance problems. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 51A: M45–M52, 1996. doi: 10.1093/gerona/51A.2.M45. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Swallow M. Fibre size and content of the anterior tibial nerve of the foot. J Neurol Neurosurg Psychiatry 29: 205–213, 1966. doi: 10.1136/jnnp.29.3.205. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Swash M, Fox KP. The effect of age on human skeletal muscle. Studies of the morphology and innervation of muscle spindles. J Neurol Sci 16: 417–432, 1972. doi: 10.1016/0022-510X(72)90048-2. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Teasdale N, Simoneau M. Attentional demands for postural control: the effects of aging and sensory reintegration. Gait Posture 14: 203–210, 2001. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(01)00134-5. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Terao S, Sobue G, Hashizume Y, Li M, Inagaki T, Mitsuma T. Age-related changes in human spinal ventral horn cells with special reference to the loss of small neurons in the intermediate zone: a quantitative analysis. Acta Neuropathol 92: 109–114, 1996. doi: 10.1007/s004010050497. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Thompson C, Bélanger M, Fung J. Effects of bilateral Achilles tendon vibration on postural orientation and balance during standing. Clin Neurophysiol 118: 2456–2467, 2007. doi: 10.1016/j.clinph.2007.08.013. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Tokuno CD, Taube W, Cresswell AG. An enhanced level of motor cortical excitability during the control of human standing. Acta Physiol (Oxf) 195: 385–395, 2009. doi: 10.1111/j.1748-1716.2008.01898.x. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Toledo DR, Barela JA. Sensory and motor differences between young and older adults: somatosensory contribution to postural control. Rev Bras Fisioter 14: 267–275, 2010. doi: 10.1590/S1413-35552010000300004. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Toledo DR, Barela JA, Kohn AF. Improved proprioceptive function by application of subsensory electrical noise: Effects of aging and task-demand. Neuroscience 358: 103–114, 2017. doi: 10.1016/j.neuroscience.2017.06.045. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Toosizadeh N, Ehsani H, Miramonte M, Mohler J. Proprioceptive impairments in high fall risk older adults: the effect of mechanical calf vibration on postural balance. Biomed Eng Online 17: 51, 2018. doi: 10.1186/s12938-018-0482-8. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Tracy BL, Enoka RM. Older adults are less steady during submaximal isometric contractions with the knee extensor muscles. J Appl Physiol (1985) 92: 1004–1012, 2002. doi: 10.1152/japplphysiol.00954.2001. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Valenzuela T, Okubo Y, Woodbury A, Lord SR, Delbaere K. Adherence to technology-based exercise programs in older adults: a systematic review. J Geriatr Phys Ther 41: 49–61, 2018. doi: 10.1519/JPT.0000000000000095. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • van Diest M, Lamoth CJ, Stegenga J, Verkerke GJ, Postema K. Exergaming for balance training of elderly: state of the art and future developments. J Neuroeng Rehabil 10: 101, 2013. doi: 10.1186/1743-0003-10-101. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Van Impe A, Bruijn SM, Coxon JP, Wenderoth N, Sunaert S, Duysens J, Swinnen SP. Age-related neural correlates of cognitive task performance under increased postural load. Age (Dordr) 35: 2111–2124, 2013. doi: 10.1007/s11357-012-9499-2. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Van Impe A, Coxon JP, Goble DJ, Doumas M, Swinnen SP. White matter fractional anisotropy predicts balance performance in older adults. Neurobiol Aging 33: 1900–1912, 2012. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2011.06.013. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Vaughan SK, Stanley OL, Valdez G. Impact of aging on proprioceptive sensory neurons and intrafusal muscle fibers in mice. J Gerontol A Biol Sci Med Sci 72: 771–779, 2017. doi: 10.1093/gerona/glw175. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Vouriot A, Gauchard GC, Chau N, Benamghar L, Lepori ML, Mur JM, Perrin PP. Sensorial organisation favouring higher visual contribution is a risk factor of falls in an occupational setting. Neurosci Res 48: 239–247, 2004. doi: 10.1016/j.neures.2003.11.001. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Wade MG, Lindquist R, Taylor JR, Treat-Jacobson D. Optical flow, spatial orientation, and the control of posture in the elderly. J Gerontol B Psychol Sci Soc Sci 50B: P51–P58, 1995. doi: 10.1093/geronb/50B.1.P51. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Wannstedt GT, Herman RM. Use of augmented sensory feedback to achieve symmetrical standing. Phys Ther 58: 553–559, 1978. doi: 10.1093/ptj/58.5.553. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Wise AK, Gregory JE, Proske U. Detection of movements of the human forearm during and after co-contractions of muscles acting at the elbow joint. J Physiol 508: 325–330, 1998. doi: 10.1111/j.1469-7793.1998.325br.x. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Wolpert DM, Kawato M. Multiple paired forward and inverse models for motor control. Neural Netw 11: 1317–1329, 1998. doi: 10.1016/S0893-6080(98)00066-5. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Woollacott M, Shumway-Cook A. Attention and the control of posture and gait: a review of an emerging area of research. Gait Posture 16: 1–14, 2002. doi: 10.1016/S0966-6362(01)00156-4. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Yeh TT, Cluff T, Balasubramaniam R. Visual reliance for balance control in older adults persists when visual information is disrupted by artificial feedback delays. PLoS One 9: e91554, 2014. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0091554. [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Zou L, Han J, Li C, Yeung AS, Hui SS, Tsang WW, Ren Z, Wang L. Effects of Tai Chi on lower limb proprioception in adults aged over 55: a systematic review and meta-analysis. Arch Phys Med Rehabil 100: 1102–1113, 2019. doi: 10.1016/j.apmr.2018.07.425. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]
  • Zwergal A, Linn J, Xiong G, Brandt T, Strupp M, Jahn K. Aging of human supraspinal locomotor and postural control in fMRI. Neurobiol Aging 33: 1073–1084, 2012. doi: 10.1016/j.neurobiolaging.2010.09.022. [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]


Articles from Journal of Neurophysiology are provided here courtesy of American Physiological Society