Uncompromised acceptance of any/all viewpoints is the surest sign of cognitive maturity.

  • Journal List
  • PLoS One
  • PMC8189483

PLoS One. 2021; 16(6): e0252913.

Wen Lin Teh, Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Funding acquisition, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Resources, Software, Validation, Visualization, Writing – original draft, Writing – review & editing,1,* Ellaisha Samari, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Project administration, Software, Writing – review & editing,1 Laxman Cetty, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Project administration, Writing – review & editing,1 Roystonn Kumarasan, Data curation, Formal analysis, Investigation, Methodology, Writing – review & editing,1 Fiona Devi, Conceptualization, Data curation, Formal analysis, Methodology, Writing – review & editing,1 Shazana Shahwan, Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Methodology, Supervision, Writing – review & editing,1 Nisha Chandwani, Conceptualization, Data curation, Investigation, Project administration, Resources, Writing – review & editing,2 and Mythily Subramaniam, Conceptualization, Funding acquisition, Resources, Supervision, Validation, Writing – review & editing1

Stephan Doering, Editor

Abstract

Illness perceptions form a key part of common-sense models which are used widely to explain variations in patient behaviours in healthcare. Despite the pervasiveness of depressive disorders worldwide and in young adults, illness perceptions of depressive disorders have not yet been well understood. Moreover, while a high proportion of cases of depressive disorders reside in South-east Asia, few have explored illness perceptions that are culturally relevant to this region. To address these limitations, this study aimed to understand illness perceptions of young adults diagnosed with depressive disorders. Face-to-face semi-structured interviews were conducted among Chinese, Malay, and Indian young adults aged 20 to 35 years old, who were seeking treatment at a psychiatric hospital. Data reached saturation after 33 interviews (10 to 12 interviews per ethnic group) and five themes emerged from the thematic analysis: 1) A reduced state of being experienced at a point of goal disengagement, 2) the accumulation of chronic stressors in a system that demands success and discourages the pursuit of personally meaningful goals, 3) a wide range of symptoms that are uncontrollable and disabling, 4) poor decision making resulting in wasted opportunities, with some positive takeaways, and 5) accepting the chronicity of depression. Young adults typically experienced depression as a reduced state of being and it was thought of cognitively as an entity that may be a part of or separate from the self. Over and beyond these aspects of cognitive representations was the emergence of themes depicting conflicts and dilemmas between the self and the social environment that threatened self-identity and autonomy. Addressing these conflicts in therapy would therefore be of utmost relevance for young adults recovering from depressive disorders in the local setting.

Introduction

The term illness evokes not only the experience of symptoms and suffering, but also the judgments, interpretations, coping, and help-seeking behaviours that go with it [1]. The construction of illness, as explained by the theory of social constructionism, is shaped, and influenced by sociocultural forces, providing social meaning to the experience of a disease [2]. In the context of illness and health, illness narratives serve as the most instinctive way of communicating the social construction of diseases [1, 3].

Illness narratives form a key part of explanatory models or common-sense models, which are used widely to explain variations in patient behaviours in healthcare. Examples of such models include the health belief model [4], the theory of planned behaviour [5], and the theory of self-efficacy [6]. One popular model is the Common-Sense Model of Self-Regulation (CSM) by Leventhal, which posits that individuals are active problem-solvers who try to understand their illness so as to cope with it [7–9]. Individuals do so by formulating their own set of cognitive representations of illness to self-regulate and manage health threats associated with their illness [7]. The central aspect of CSM is the use of ‘lay’ beliefs to predict health-promoting or coping behaviours.

According to the model, illness representations of any illness comprise of at least five dimensions: 1) Identity: symptoms that are perceived to be part of the illness, 2) Time-line: acute or chronic in duration, 3) Consequences: the extent of the impact on individuals’ lives, 4) Causes: whether the illness is due to internal or external causes, and 5) Cure or control: the extent to which the illness is curable or incurable. The model also consists of two independent processes (cognitive and emotional) that deal with the representation of fear or danger of illness threats which trigger coping mechanisms and outcome appraisals thereafter providing feedback to the model [7]. The CSM is one of the most widely used, due to its good psychometric properties and applicability in predicting self-care behaviours and recovery in physical illnesses [10, 11].

Increasingly, CSM is being applied to mental illness research as a way to understand and predict patient behaviours. Unlike other health belief models, CSM comprises of emotional illness representations, which are directly relevant to mental illnesses, making it a useful guide [12]. In their review article, Lobban and colleagues detailed numerous studies that were consistent with the CSM and had described how the model is able to explain and fit the mental illness experience [12].

However, several authors have highlighted limitations over the applicability of CSM in mental illness as opposed to physical illnesses [12–14]. While CSM is a useful guide, it has been criticized to be overly simplistic in explaining illness representations of mental illnesses [12, 15]. Unlike physical illnesses, the cause-and-effect associations in mental illnesses are far less clear as there are greater variations in the causal beliefs and experiences of mental illnesses. Individuals often struggle to accept mental illness [13, 16, 17]. Coming to terms with the illness is neither straightforward nor coherent [13, 18], such that individuals may express different illness beliefs simultaneously even if those beliefs contradict each other [14]. Moreover, unlike the case of physical illnesses where there is a clear distinction between the self and illness, the boundary between mental illness and the self is far less clear and often intersects. As a result, individuals with mental illnesses do not always see their symptoms in a negative light. Higbed and Fox’s qualitative investigation into thirteen patients with anorexia nervosa, reported that individuals who felt the condition was a meaningful part of their identity were reluctant to do away with the illness [19]. Pedley and colleagues’ qualitative investigation into sixteen people with obsessive-compulsive disorder found that participants perceived their symptoms positively as a sort of peculiarity in their personality, rather than viewing them negatively as symptoms of an illness [20]. Thus, unlike physical illnesses, mental illness symptoms are not always undesired and this has crucial ethical implications in the context of treating mental disorders where the patients’ definitions of recovery are often overlooked.

Yet consolidating an explanatory model for mental illness remains a challenge as patient narratives differ considerably by the type of illness. Perceptions of more common forms of mental illnesses, such as depression or anxiety, are more varied as compared to less common forms, such as schizophrenia [14], with the former conditions being more dependent on what is considered to be culturally accepted expressions of “distress” [14]. For instance, Asian patients with depression tend to describe illness complaints as physical rather than psychological, such as reporting to feel ‘tired’ rather than ‘sad’, since the psychological labelling of symptoms is often stigmatized in non-western cultures [21]. In another example, mental illness is more likely to be attributed to a ‘weakness of character’ in Japanese individuals as compared to Australian individuals, who are more likely to attribute it to physiological reasons [22]. These examples depict the role of culture, its influence and interaction with personal beliefs in the social construction and experience of illness [23].

Till date, the majority of qualitative work have been conducted amongst severe but less common forms of mental illnesses, such as psychosis or schizophrenia [12, 14], while qualitative investigations into more common forms of mental illnesses, such as depressive disorders are few and far between. Within the context of depressive disorders, the majority of existing studies have been conducted among individuals with ongoing primary physical conditions who also report depressive symptoms, patients who meet criteria for depression based on self-report and cut-off scores, or among healthcare professionals [24–27]. Yet, only a few have been conducted amongst individuals who are formally diagnosed with depressive disorders and who are seeking psychiatric care [28, 29].

The World Health Organization (WHO) estimates that approximately 4.4% of individuals suffer from depression worldwide [30]. According to the same report, the majority of cases reside in South-east Asia (27% of cases), followed by the Western Pacific region (21% of cases), Eastern Mediterranean region (16% of cases), the Americas (15% of cases), European (12% of cases), and African regions (9% of cases) [30], yet few studies have explored illness perceptions that are culturally relevant to this region. Furthermore, understanding the culture surrounding young adulthood ensures that mental health services stay relevant to their needs; needs which have grown more complex today than ever before [31]. Young adulthood is arguably the period where an individual is most productive in life and from an economic standpoint, mental illness can pose a significant threat to productivity and disease burden. This is crucial for Singapore as human capital is considered to be an important asset [32]. Major Depressive Disorder (MDD) is the most pervasive disorder among young adults aged 18 to 34 years in Singapore [33] and has a significant treatment gap [34]. Thus, understanding how illness is experienced among young adults would be important for interventions at the initial stages of disease onset. To the best of our knowledge, there has been no qualitative inquiry into illness perceptions of depression that is specific to a non-western psychiatric population in Singapore and while it is typically assumed that non-western communities have more spiritual/traditional narratives of mental illness [21, 22, 35], a highly globalized multi-ethnic society such as Singapore may present illness narratives that are culturally distinct; it is not uncommon for Singaporeans to simultaneously utilize both allopathic and traditional forms of treatment in a multifarious medical system [36–38].

The present study aims to explore illness representations in individuals with depressive disorders in Singapore. The two main research questions are: 1) What are the illness perceptions of young persons with depression in Singapore? and 2) How does the role of culture influence illness perceptions in young people diagnosed with depression locally?

While the nature of our inquiry is exploratory, we would expect illness perceptions to be highly associated with the self. Additionally, these dimensions are also expected to be more complex, reflecting a combination of both allopathic and culturally derived concepts of illness representation.

Methods

Procedure

Convenient and purposive sampling were utilized for recruitment. Recruitment was conducted in two ways: individuals were primarily referred to the research team by their clinicians during their outpatient visit or inpatient stay or were approached at the outpatient clinics by a member of the research team with information flyers that described the study. The inclusion criteria: Singapore citizens or permanent residents, aged 18 to 35 years, with a Diagnostic and Statistical Manual, Fourth Edition (DSM-IV) diagnosis of depressive disorder, receiving outpatient treatment at the Institute of Mental Health (IMH), and were able to speak, read, and write in English. The exclusion criteria: patients with a diagnosis of substance-induced depressive disorders, depressive disorder due to a general medical condition, post-natal depression, bipolar disorder, and depression with psychotic symptoms.

All consent taking procedures (both verbal and written consent) were conducted by members of the research team and not by their referring clinicians to prevent coercion. Participants who verbally agreed to be interviewed were re-contacted within a few days but no longer than a week later via phone contact. Patients were then asked a second time over the phone if they were willing to be interviewed. This ensured that participants had sufficient time and space to reconsider their participation. After the second verbal consent was given, the researcher scheduled an interview appointment with the participant. Inpatients who agreed to the study were interviewed after they were discharged. Written consent was obtained from all participants before the start of the interviews.

As the communication styles of individuals in Asian cultures differ substantially from Western cultures, one-to-one interviews were preferred over focus group discussions [39]. To minimize the possibility that participants may describe their illness beliefs in medical terms, the interviews were carried out at the research interview rooms which were situated away from the clinical setting in IMH. Only one interview was conducted outside of the hospital at the convenience of the participant. To minimize the potential lack of cultural sensitivity that may present itself during the interviews, interviewees were matched with interviewers according to ethnicity as much as possible. An interview schedule (S1 File) that covered questions on the depression experience and culture was used. Interviews were conducted primarily in English; however, as participants were bilingual, they were encouraged to converse in their mother tongue languages if they felt more comfortable doing so. Interviews were audio-recorded and transcribed verbatim in both English and mother tongue languages (Chinese/Malay/Tamil) which were translated in the same transcript. Interviews lasted approximately 1 hour and 3 minutes on average, and the duration of the interviews ranged from approximately 33 minutes to 2 hours.

This study was approved by the local institutional ethics and review boards: the Domain Specific Review Board (DSRB) of the National Health Group (NHG) and the Institutional Research Review Committee (IRRC) in IMH.

Participants

A total of 52 individuals were approached from February 2018 to January 2019. Fourteen of those approached had refused to participate or were uncontactable when re-contacted. Most individuals did not cite any reason for refusing, while some others mentioned that they were uninterested, busy, or uncomfortable with the study’s interview format. Individuals signed written informed consent forms before the start of the interviews. Diagnoses were initially based on self-report or by a referring clinician (NS, who knew the inclusion and exclusion criteria for this study) and were only verified after the completion of the study with their electronic medical records (upon getting written consent to do so) or their doctors. Four participants were withdrawn as they did not fulfil the eligibility criteria. Thus, a total of 34 individuals (19 females and 15 males) with depressive disorders completed the interviews. Subsequently, one transcript was excluded from the analysis as it differed substantially from the rest of the transcripts (participant’s illness beliefs were primarily associated with another medical condition), leaving a final count of 33 transcripts. Participants were purposively sampled to ensure adequate representation by ethnic groups. Data reached saturation after 12 Chinese, 11 Malay, and 9 Indian participants were interviewed. Although one participant was Sri Lankan, she had been living in Singapore most of her life. Her narrative met with great familiarity and was analysed with the rest of the transcripts. The average age was 26 years old and ranged from 20 to 35 years old at the point of the interview. Participants were on average living with depressive disorders for approximately 3.7 years, ranging from 4 months to 16 years. In terms of highest educational qualifications attained, 12 participants had pre-university or secondary school qualifications and 21 participants had a diploma, university degree, or vocational qualifications. All of the participant characteristics are summarized in Table 1.

Table 1

Self-reported participant characteristics and sociodemographic information (n = 33).

S/NAgeSexEthnicityYears1Highest educationMarital StatusReligionWork Status
C01 34 F Chinese 8 Polytechnic Diploma Single Buddhism Unemployed
C02 20 F Chinese <1 A’ level/Completed Pre-U or Junior College Single Christianity Student
C04 30 M Chinese 2 Polytechnic Diploma Single Buddhism Full-time
C05 34 F Chinese 7 University degree Single Christianity Part-time
C06 26 M Chinese 8 O/N level/ Completed Secondary education Single Christianity Part-time
C07 24 F Chinese <1 Polytechnic Diploma Single Taoism Part-time
C08 31 F Chinese 3.5 University degree Single Christianity Unemployed
C09 35 F Chinese <1 Polytechnic Diploma Married Buddhism Full-time
C10 22 F Chinese 4 A’ level/Completed Pre-U or Junior College Single Christianity Student
C11 22 M Chinese 2 A’ level/Completed Pre-U or Junior College Single Buddhism Student
C12 22 F Chinese 2^ A’ level/Completed Pre-U or Junior College Single Buddhism Student
C13 20 M Chinese 3 A’ level/Completed Pre-U or Junior College Single Free-thinker Other
L01 34 F Indian 16 University degree Single Hinduism Part-time
L02 27 M Indian 7^ Polytechnic Diploma Single Buddhism Part-time
L03 27 F Indian 4 University degree Single Hinduism Unemployed
L05 21 M Indian 1 O/N level/ Completed Secondary education Single Free-thinker Other
L06 26 M Indian 2 Polytechnic Diploma Single Christianity Unemployed
L08 22 F Indian <1 Polytechnic Diploma Single Free-thinker Full-time
L09 26 F Indian 8 University degree Single Islam Full-time
L10 25 M Indian 2 Polytechnic Diploma Single Others Student
L11 23 M Indian 7 O/N level/ Completed Secondary education Single Christianity Unemployed
L12 22 F Sri Lankan 6 O/N level/ Completed Secondary education Single Free-thinker Student
M01 26 M Malay 3 Polytechnic Diploma Single Islam Full-time
M02 23 M Malay 1 Polytechnic Diploma Single Islam Full-time
M03 27 M Malay <1 Polytechnic Diploma Single Islam Full-time
M04 21 M Malay 1 Vocational Institute/ ITE Nitec Cert Single Islam Student
M05 28 M Malay 2 University degree Single Islam Other
M06 22 F Malay 4 Polytechnic Diploma Single Islam Part-time
M07 25 F Malay <1 O/N level/ Completed Secondary education Single Free-thinker Full-time
M09 35 F Malay 2 Polytechnic Diploma Single Others Full-time
M10 29 F Malay 2 O/N level/ Completed Secondary education Single Islam Full-time
M11 31 F Malay 3 O/N level/ Completed Secondary education Married Islam Full-time
M12 26 F Malay 1 Polytechnic Diploma Single Islam Full-time

Analysis

We chose to analyse the transcripts with thematic analysis described by Clarke and Braun [40, 41] because of the advantages it offers to researchers working in the healthcare setting. Thematic analysis allows researchers in healthcare to overcome methodological constraints, such as a lack of time, resources, and expertise that are required for many other qualitative approaches. The authors (WLT, ES, LC, RK, FD, SS) are all quantitative researchers by formal training (i.e., degrees in psychology/sociology at least), have all been trained in qualitative research, and are experienced in the use of thematic analysis.

The initial coding process involved conducting open coding of each sentence in each transcript [42, 43]. The first author utilized deductive (themes of CSM were determined a priori) and inductive (from ground up) coding simultaneously throughout the coding process using the framework described by Feredey and Muir-Cochrane in 2006 [44]. Inductive coding involved recognizing, identifying, and clustering of important information that represented aspects of a phenomenon. On the other hand, deductive coding involved using pre-existing codes as a guide and template for the clustering of information. Transcripts were analysed using the Nvivo software.

All transcripts were analysed thematically by the first author (WLT) to determine if 1) data reached theoretical saturation for each ethnic group and if 2) there were new emergent codes or themes. The initial process of coding started at the midpoint of the study as interviews were being conducted. Transcripts were initially analysed by ethnicity, but as the preliminary themes did not differ substantially between ethnic groups, transcripts were eventually analysed as a whole. A codebook that comprised of the codes and its’ definitions was reviewed and analysed iteratively through multiple discussions between the team members (WLT, ES, LC, RK, FD, SS) until a high inter-rater agreement of 0.73 (Kappa) was achieved. Co-author MS provided advice and guidance over the study design, procedures, and analyses of the study. Interviews ceased when no new information, codes, or themes emerged from the interviews that could change the codebook, in other words, data had reached theoretical saturation [43]. The first author also wrote memos detailing her immediate thoughts of possible links to theoretical concepts during the entire thematic process, which were later discussed alongside the review of the codebook with team members.

Results

The themes that emerged were generally in accordance with the CSM as determined a priori. However, additional themes emerged from inductive coding which were separate from the concept of CSM. In summary, five major themes emerged: 1) A reduced state of being experienced at a point of goal disengagement, 2) The accumulation of chronic stressors in a system that demands success and discourages the pursuit of personally meaningful goals, 3) A wide range of symptoms that are uncontrollable and disabling, 4) Poor decision making resulting in wasted opportunities, with some positive takeaways, and 5) Accepting the chronicity of depression.

1. A reduced state of being experienced at a point of goal disengagement

1.1 At a point of disengagement of existing (unattainable) goals

Depression was likened to “a stop button in my life.” It was experienced as a point of change or disengagement from the lack of achievement of existing goals and expectations.

“It has put like a stop on life for me. I’m, I’ve stopped going to school, I’ve stopped just doing anything I liked, and just kept to myself at home. It just, yeah it was like a stop button in my life.” O12/22/F/SL

“…So I would say prior to when I was depressed I was kind of interested in joining the public service so I was really kind of structuring my life, my interests, a lot of things lah1 to fit within that idealized career or mine… It really changed my whole direction in life lah1. For now, I really, I really don’t want to join the public service… I’ve just completely given up that dream.” C11/22/M/C

1lah and loh are Singlish (Singapore English) colloquial words that function as discourse particles that set the tone of conversations (usually to stress a point) but do not alter the meaning of sentences.

It manifested cognitively as a lack of direction (i.e., feeling lost and stuck) and motivation (i.e., having no point in life), and affectively as frustration and discontentment.

“Or…oh ya like feeling trapped or like they do the job, same thing every day like the life not going anywhere kind of thing.” M02/23/M/M

“…you fail to see the meaning of a lot of things like…maybe there’s no reason to live, there’s no reason to work, there’s no point in doing all these because…nothing will come out of it…” I06/26/M/I

1.2. An entity that is separate or a part of the self

Depression was described to be associated with the self in two ways. First, depression was seen as a separate entity that endangered the sense of self. Some participants were constantly battling depression in their minds, preventing it from encroaching into their sense of selves. Experiencing a depressive episode was thus seen as being powerless over the hold that depression had over them which they depicted as an indomitable force.

“I’m constantly fighting my demons so I’m very small and my demons are this big. And they’re loud and they’re strong so on bad days I was just tell my friends lah like my demons are here today I don’t wanna talk to anyone. So that’s how I relate my bad thoughts and and and… they too strong for me sometimes that I can’t even hear my own voice” M07/25/F

“No actually it’s not really about being sad… So [the thing in] the mind is taking over your body… So you can’t, you can’t actually fight whatever that you’re feeling. So you just let the feeling come and stay for a while…” M11/31/F/

Depression had a lasting impact on self-identity. Over time, it displaced the self.

“…I don’t even know myself anymore… ‘cause all my friends all say … “why you become like that, errr I want the old [C07] back’ but every single time they say that I don’t even recall how I was last time. I don’t even feel like human… As time pass it just get… it just eats you up…” C07/24/F/C

Second, depression was seen as part of the self by some participants, primarily as an emotion that everyone experiences in varying degrees.

“Because depression, in itself, is an emotion that everyone goes through. Yeah, but they may be able to get out again before it becomes like a mental health condition per say. “I09/26/F/I

As these participants perceived symptoms of depression to be universally present, the meaning of “having depression” had more to do with taking on the medical label rather than the presence of it.

“…it’s just the more depressed me, side of me talking…I think I actually had it already depression. As in there wasn’t a name to it when I had it… I’m like oh yeah, like it’s an official medical term to it. Then ok, I have it.” C12/22/F/C

Reactions to the medical label were mixed. Being clinically diagnosed was reassuring as the feelings that they had been dealing with for a long period of time were legitimized, yet at the same time, there was a sense of guilt as having the medical label provided a justification for a lack of ‘control’ over these symptoms.

“Sometimes it can be comforting knowing that there is a, I would say medical term to it… Sometimes when I feel like I told you, feel lazy about it. Ok just let it be. And let the depression sink in. So, sometimes when you have a medical term to it where you know what your diagnosed with, you your own self might take it, take advantage of the term that you have and you let yourself spiral down also.” C12/22/F/C

1.3. A reduced state of being

Regardless of whether depression was seen as separate from or as a part of the self, participants generally believed that a depressive episode was a reduced state of being that eventuated from a depletion of cognitive resources that were used to deal with external (i.e., environmental or social) afflictions which was usually narrated to be chronic in nature. In other words, depression is experienced when the mind was no longer “strong enough” to withstand periods of accumulated stress.

“I was still like high functioning kind of thing but perhaps they just went on uh untreated for too long and the various disorders and symptoms or syndromes just kept accumulating and getting more and more serious you know so perhaps it’s like a, like maybe my brain just can’t take it any more lah that kind of thing…” C08/31/F/C

“I think it makes sense that it should be a… it would be (an) accumulation of factors because we’re all build in a certain amount of resilient you know and then when it just gets too much then you become depressed I suppose…” I01/34/F/I

2. The accumulation of chronic stressors in a system that demands success and discourages the pursuit of personally meaningful goals

2.1. Causes are multi-faceted and often chronic in nature

While participants acknowledged that there are multiple biological, environmental, and social causes, which could interact and lead to depression, participants reiterated that a large part of it was due to an accumulation of chronic environmental or social stressors. Family was frequently seen as a main source of stress, conflict, instability, and abuse which panned out over a long period of time.

“… I think when we were 13 the whole lot of us including the first wife and his 5 sons were in like a house together and I was just like trying so hard to be happy, like playing games with them but I know this isn’t right you know. Like they’re constantly being so impulsive and like running and making the wrong decisions and impacting everyone under them.” M07/25/F

As a result of the chronic lack of warmth and closeness with the family, some individuals became heavily and emotionally invested in relationships with significant others. Unfortunately, when these relationships dissolved, it became overwhelming, triggering episodes of depression.

“… So when we separated, part ways, um I guess my emotions…I needed emotional support elsewhere. Because she was my support… not my mum, not my dad, not my sister…just her. So I felt myself going down and down and down and down until to the point I couldn’t cope, I couldn’t eat. You know because I was so reliant on her.” I10/25/M/I

Additionally, work and school were cited as common sources of stress. Individuals were expected to perform or conform to a strict and highly pressured work culture or were overwhelmed by the demands of school. The inability to leave the chronic stressor (i.e., “They don’t have a choice”) and the lack of timely resolution was perceived to lead to an accumulation of stress past a threshold and into depression.

“Six o’clock… is the end of the day, nobody can leave at 6. There’s once I leave at 6:15, they call me back just to do another work because I leave too early.” C07/24/F/C

“…it’s being constantly uhm, being put into a circumstance or a position where you’re not, one thing is that you’re not happy. For a very, very long time and suddenly that becomes the new normal?” M05/28/M/M

2.2. High societal expectations of success

The “chase of achievements” or being an “overachiever” at school or work is a societal expectation and a norm that is instilled in the family since young. It is the “system that demands that I do well” (C11/22/M). This mentality was seen as damaging and was believed to be a main contributor of depression for some. Much of the content in negative thinking (i.e., feeling useless, shame) appeared to be associated with the lack of accomplishment.

“…The chase for achievements that you know that is very part of how we function as a country. Like most of the yah you know like from young it’s tuition and whatever and like all sorts of enrichment classes and all that yah so like yah so like but actually that kind of mind-mentality yah … for someone with depression especially someone who has been you know always pushing and perhaps it’s the pushing thing that just uh, that could be one of the causes…” C08/31/F/C

“… I didn’t want to go to school, I didn’t want to face exams. Then, I felt that if I didn’t do the exams then, because at that time they say that if you do very well in your exams, get a good job and everything, then I felt that if I didn’t go and face exams, I mean I won’t be able to, I won’t be able to clear the, get the A levels ah. Then I feel that I’m very useless, then I feel that maybe I should just die or something… I felt a sense of shame in myself. I cannot push myself to go and take the exams and I think, I think a lot of negative thoughts came back” C06/26/M/C

“… Like people’s expectations and then when we cannot meet it, we feel like we failed, that constant feeling of failure over the years could… It just seems like something traumatic I guess, less intense but it builds over time, or like it’s some sort of traumatic experience, bullying abuse, that kind of thing.” I05/21/F/I

2.3. Unable to pursue personal goals that are incongruent with familial and societal expectations

According to some participants, depression was experienced when they felt they had failed to reach their goals. Living up to high familial/societal expectations was difficult, yet switching to goals that were personally more meaningful was not seen as an option for many. Not being able to define and pursue personal goals, especially goals that were incongruent with the expectations of the family or society, were articulated as a contributor to depression. The lack of agency in influencing the direction of their future that is personally meaningful left them feeling demotivated and trapped at the same time.

“So yeah it’s every day studying something you have no interest in, and then knowing you’re never going to use it? Or if you do it’s going to be in a job that you don’t like. So it sort of, just made me feel very down, miserable about it, and sort of angry at everybody that I was wasting my time when I could be learning something I felt passionate about.” I05/21/M/I

“I think it’s a sense of, don’t have any motivation to do anything in life. Don’t have a goal. Just do everything because… I don’t see meaning in doing anything, I don’t have a goal.” C06/26/M/C

“…all my life you know throughout my secondary school I just never did what I wanted to la. Like the CCA I wanted to, I wanted to do a third language I also wasn’t allowed to. So my life was kinda decided for me you know kinda thing … I would lose interest in it in fact you know and I just become disinterested. I still have that a bit now ya but at that point it was more pronounced la. So you know I just didn’t had the courage to stand up to them (family) I guess.” I01/34/F/I

3. A wide range of symptoms that are uncontrollable and disabling

Participants mentioned somatic, psychological, cognitive, and emotional symptoms as indicators of depression that were disabling in nature. Participants cited somatic symptoms most often. These symptoms included sleeping issues such as insomnia, hypersomnia, low appetite, low energy, fatigue, or migraines. Behavioural symptoms such as self-harm and attempted suicide were also mentioned but less frequently.

“If not one day you don’t want to be awake. You just…you just don’t want to do anything. You just want to sleep throughout your entire life.” I01/34/F/I

“I never experienced the sadness symptoms at all, for me it’s just I just feel tired loh1 exhausted loh or empty or I feel nothing.” C08/31/F/C

“Low energy is constant; I feel this all the time, low self-esteem” C10/22/F/C

In addition to behavioural and somatic symptoms, participants commonly cited cognitive symptoms. The two types of cognitive symptoms that were typically present in the narratives: 1) Cognitive symptoms that were overly active, uncontrollable, and intrusive (i.e. rumination or catastrophic thinking), and 2) cognitive aspects which were absent or reduced (i.e. lacking motivation, lacking concentration, helplessness or hopelessness).

“I don’t know why my memory, my brain very active. I try to like, it’s very tiring cause it’s a, I told you it’s a loop. So, legit, the scene will just play out very fast…” C12/22/F/C

“…the ability to concentrate…um, the self-confidence that you have in yourself and this sense of satisfaction which I feel that depression takes away… Even after completing a very large project, I don’t actually feel that sense of satisfaction which makes it very hard for me to keep going and do like the next thing and the next thing because I never feel fulfilled, or never feel satisfied or this sense of achievement.” I09/26/F/I

Participants described psychological and emotional symptoms as well, such as feeling sad, crying, anxiety and hallucinations, but were mentioned less frequently.

4. Poor decision making resulting in wasted opportunities, with some positive takeaways

For the majority of participants, depression had a negative impact on their general and social functioning, and school or work performances. In addition, some participants recounted making poor decisions on key instances which adversely affected their future outlook.

4.1. Consequences from poor decision making and wasted opportunities

Several participants felt that because of depression, they held themselves back from key opportunities that would otherwise improve future prospects if they had seized them. Interestingly, not being able to take on opportunities was indicative of being separate from normal.

“I will say it makes me more like…it makes me feel like I am separated from others and I don’t really have the opportunities that other people have. Opportunities more like I am holding myself back, that kind of like my school has this erm…not event more like a group called the [PROGRAMME] where it’s basically like all the people with high GPA go into and …like because of depression, I can’t really put myself in there” M04/21/M/M

4.2. Positive consequences

Contrastingly, a few participants believed that there were positive consequences. One participant believed depression gave her a sense of empathy and advocacy for others with mental illnesses.

“… I could kind of erm empathize with them to a high degree because I could look at the person and say look you know I understand what you going through and really advocate for them, push for them without any judgment you know and I think that definitely helped a lot so my depression would certainly have helped in that…” I01/34/F/I

5. Accepting the chronic nature of depression

Participants had initially believed that depression was an acute condition that could be cured within a short amount of time but most have come to accept that depression is a chronic condition, and the route to recovery is long and arduous.

“…actually last time, I thought that depression will last for like a few years, but now I’ve actually accept that it will last uh for a lifetime. Yah. Because uh not matter how we fight it, if the person around me keeps on giving you negativity, you will still stick to your depression. If the things that triggers in the first place just won’t go off, then the sickness will always stay…” M11/31/F

“Like all along I kind of uh was viewing this whole uhm journey of you know journeying through my various health conditions and all I kind of just viewed as it like a phase and I just can’t wait for it to be over. But when I saw that (an Instagram post) and like, it kind of spoke to me in that yah perhaps uhm it’s actually even ok if this so called dark period is not just a period, but just… there. I would just be in this darkness uh… for as long as it is." C08/31/F/C

Depression was likened to cancer. The majority of participants did not believe that depression could be eradicated completely or that they could be fully cured of its symptoms (i.e. “Not a hundred percent curable”). Depression could be managed or treated but the chance of relapse was thought of as highly likely due to unforeseen stressors that may crop up in the future.

“As far as I’m concerned it’s something that I’ll have for the rest of my life and I just, at this point I’m just managing it at the moment” M05/28/M/M

“I don’t know if there is a cure for it right. I mean, I don’t know. There’s only the treatment I guess and it’s like cancer, can come back anytime” M10/29/F

From the narratives, depression was thought of cognitively as an entity that may be a part of or separate from the self. Regardless of cognitive representations, depression was primarily experienced as a reduced state of being that occurred as a result of a depletion of resources to cope with unresolved and often chronic stressors. Such chronic stressors typically comprised having to deal with prolonged periods of strained relations with the family, or demands from school or work. Having depression meant making poor decisions on key instances which participants perceived to have had profound long-term consequences. Further, depression was seen as a chronic condition with no means of a full recovery. Over and beyond these aspects of cognitive representations was the emergence of themes surrounding goal disengagement and re-engagement which was unique to the present narratives.

Discussion

This study aimed to explore illness perceptions in young adults with depressive disorders in Singapore. The themes that emerged from this study were to an extent similar to past literature supporting the claim that the CSM has its limits in modelling the way individuals perceive mental illnesses due to added dimensions associated with the self [12, 14]. Along with these results, there were also emergent themes that were unique to depressive disorders which will be discussed at greater length in the present section.

Depression, self-identity, and goal disengagement

Based on the themes that emerged from inductive open coding, depression was highly associated with the self and self-concepts. Our findings support the notion that there might be an added dimension of illness perception regarding the extent to which the condition is seen as a part of or separate from the self [20], and could reflect differing levels of conflict or acceptance during identity reconstruction [45, 46].

Unlike physical illnesses, which are almost always appraised as unwanted external afflictions to the body, individuals may or may not see depressive disorders in the same regard. When individuals were asked what depression meant to them, they generally described it as a psychological entity that was either separate from or a part of the self, which is consistent with past reports that investigated illness perceptions in individuals with severe types of mental illnesses [13, 14, 17, 19, 20]. Due to the realization that depression is a chronic condition with no means of full recovery, young adults saw themselves grappling with identity changes manifesting within the self [46]. Regardless, episodes of depression were often described as a reduced state of being from their normal selves. This experience of depression is similar to that found in a qualitative study by Kinderman et al. among individuals with psychosis, where episodes were considered to be “periods of socially recognized states of altered psychological functioning” [13]. However, there is a difference. According to participants, a reduced state of being was experienced as a result of a depletion of cognitive resources required to cope with the accumulation of unresolved and often chronic stressors, which is vastly different from being simply in an altered state of irrationality described by individuals with psychosis [13]. Finally, similar to past reports, depression is not always appraised as negative [19, 20]. Having depression gave new meaning and perspectives (e.g., advocacy, empathy) into the lives of a few participants.

From our findings, conflicts in self-identity are central to the experiences of depression. Constructivist theorists have posited that individuals experience implicative dilemmas, defined as a type of cognitive dissonance that arises whenever a desired change in a self-concept clashes with a system’s worldview, on which such a change is undesired [47]. Conflicts within self-concepts in relation to goal reconstruction seem to reflect a struggle to satisfy society’s desired ideals alongside personal aspirations.

Even though Singapore is often lauded as a melting pot of eastern and western cultures, the family is fundamentally collectivistic in nature. Mainstream ideals of success that are propagated from Chinese influences (the majority ethnic group) are inculcated in the family and in school. Capitalistic values of competitiveness and meritocracy, fuelled by the need to survive as a nation since the industrial era, are entrenched in the cultural fabric of Singapore [48]. From a young age, individuals are taught at home and in school to be successful and to be a useful member of society; life’s happiness is contingent on a person’s level of contribution to society which is measured by achievements and social standing. The majority of young adults recalled feeling that they were under tremendous pressure to fulfil unrealistic ideals [49] originating from the family [50]. Additionally, at least relevant for the current sample, this pressure was contributed by an achievement-focused mind-set that was heavily instilled from a young age, and consistently reminded in schools and workplaces which demand excellence and uncompromised productivity. Participants described this mind-set to be detrimental when goals became unattainable; the pressure to succeed at such goals in a system that demands success, could have contributed to the rise of more severe forms of depression [51, 52]. The perceived inability to re-engage new and personally meaningful goals could have further exacerbated and prolonged symptoms of depression past a clinical threshold [53, 54]. Moreover, it was evident that the difficulties surrounding goal re-engagement were partly due to the fear of negative social ramifications (e.g., shame, conflict, social exclusion), as the expectations of the family are almost non-negotiable in typical Asian families [55, 56]. Contemplating to reconstruct personally meaningful goals that may go against society’s ideal expectations of success seem to parallel a clash of worldviews, which in turn, can create high levels of conflict within the self [47].

Coping with goal failure is one aspect that is rarely discussed among clinical populations [51, 52]. The link between goal disengagement/engagement and the depression experience found in this study would thus warrant future research into this phenomenon. Cognitive paradigms suggest that depression can be improved by addressing the negative cognitive triad (negative view of the self, the world, and the future) in therapy [57]. Consequently, dilemma-focused therapy is as beneficial as Cognitive Behavioural Therapy (CBT) in ameliorating symptoms of depression [58]. Our results support the notion that addressing implicative dilemmas in addition to negative cognitive self-concepts is important and greatly relevant culturally for the recovery of depression in young adults in Singapore [59]. Given that the average onset of depression lies in the period of young adulthood [60], which is also a crucial period of psychosocial development that includes identity formation, self-development, and achievement goal orientations [61–65], the temporal overlap with such crucial developmental period further exacerbates the need to understand the role of culture and family in goal reconstruction in young adulthood. Discussing cultural expectations of success with the family in therapy would thus be beneficial for young adults with depression locally.

Depression illness beliefs in a multi-ethnic non-western society

Culture was initially assumed in this study as static and pre-defined identities that tie closely to ethnicity (i.e. Chinese/Malay/Indian cultures). However, the similarities in narratives across ethnic groups reflect a different definition of culture, or macro-culture, defined as how individuals actively make sense of their lives and illness through common experiences regardless of ethnic background [66]. Illness perceptions of depressive disorders were generally similar across Chinese, Malay, and Indian ethnic groups, and to that of the West [17, 18, 67]. Perhaps due to the sample’s young adult makeup that consisted of a highly educated majority (67% of participants had a diploma or university degrees), causal beliefs were neither overwhelmingly spiritual nor traditional but were highly similar to themes reported in the extant literature that also had a strong emphasis on socio-environmental and psychosocial causes [13, 18, 20, 68]. This was accompanied by a preponderance of the belief that a relapse was strongly dependent on unforeseen future socio-environmental triggers that could potentially provoke future episodes of depression.

Collectively, such beliefs are noteworthy as two inferences can be derived. First, even as young adult patients sought biomedical interventions, such as pharmacotherapy, they were perceived to work primarily as a temporary relief of symptoms rather than a panacea to rid the condition altogether [13]. Second, it is perceived that the root of the depression lies in the disharmony of the self within the system, and therefore, it cannot be fully addressed by engaging either one part in isolation. Recovery of depression should be approached by addressing both cultural and biopsychosocial factors in an integrated fashion.

An integrated approach should be emphasized as the country gains momentum in reshaping its healthcare system towards one that is patient centred. While young adults do benefit from clinical interventions, their narratives paint a broader picture of how limiting these interventions may be without considering the sociocultural context. Depression means to be in a reduced state of being, which is a frame of mind of living a less than meaningful life, characterised by a struggle to reconcile intersubjective misalignments [69] that threatens self-identity and autonomy. Recovery thus not only involves a return to premorbid health but also involves the process of regaining control over one’s agency, goals, and aspirations [70]. Our findings further support the notion that depression is caused (in part) by the strained relationship between the self and the social environment [45]. Therefore. regaining control over goals would therefore require the recognition and support from the immediate social environment [69, 71], on which a young self-identity can continually be validated and fostered.

This study is not without limitations. First, some of the participants were undergoing treatment and/or therapy at the time of the interview and it is unclear how much of illness beliefs were a result of psychoeducation. Second, we recruited young adults who were willing and able to sit through an interview. It is possible that those who refused the study, especially those who had cited that they did not feel comfortable with the interview format, may have different illness experiences. Thus, illness beliefs found in this study may differ from those who are not undergoing formal treatment or who refused the study.

On the surface, young adults’ illness representations of depression have struck a chord with past literature. Careful thematic explication revealed important sociocultural nuances that were unique to the current context. Young adults reportedly faced tremendous pressure to fulfil unrealistic societal expectations and had consequently struggled to construct personally meaningful goals. Depression is thus a frame of mind of living a less than meaningful life, which at a latent level, represented an underlying struggle of addressing implicative dilemmas that represent a clash of personal and cultural worldviews. These findings re-emphasized the role of culture in illness perceptions of depression and self-identity. Addressing societal and familial expectations in relation to goal failure would therefore have immense relevance for young adult individuals seeking psychotherapy in Singapore.

Supporting information

S1 File

Interview schedule.

(DOCX)

Funding Statement

This research is supported by the Singapore Ministry of Health’s National Medical Research Council under the Centre Grant Programme (Grant No.: NMRC/CG/M002/2017_IMH). The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Data Availability

We have indicated that the data for this study is available upon request. The restriction is imposed by our institutional and ethics committee. Data can only be shared after a proposal is approved by the ethics committee. This has also been conveyed to our participants during the consent process. The data request can be sent to The Institutional Research Review Committee, Institute of Mental Health, Singapore; Email address: gs.moc.hmi@hcraeserhmi.

References

1. Kleinman A. The illness narratives: Suffering, healing, and the human condition. New York: Basic Books, Inc., Publishers; 1988. [Google Scholar]

2. Conrad P, Barker KK. The social construction of illness: Key insights and policy implications. Journal of health social behavior. 2010;51(1_suppl):S67–S79. doi: 10.1177/0022146510383495 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

3. Mattingly C, Garro LC. Narrative and the cultural construction of illness and healing: Univ of California Press; 2000. [Google Scholar]

4. Becker MH. The health belief model and sick role behavior. Health education monographs. 1974;2(4):409–19. [Google Scholar]

5. Ajzen I. The theory of planned behaviour: Reactions and reflections. Taylor & Francis; 2011. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

6. Bandura A, Freeman W, Lightsey R. Self-efficacy: The exercise of control. Springer; 1999. [Google Scholar]

7. Robitaille C. Illness representations: theoretical foundations 1997. 19 p. [Google Scholar]

8. Cameron LD, Leventhal H. The self-regulation of health and illness behaviour: psychology press; 2003. [Google Scholar]

9. Leventhal H, Phillips LA, Burns E. The Common-Sense Model of Self-Regulation (CSM): A dynamic framework for understanding illness self-management. Journal of behavioral medicine. 2016;39(6):935–46. doi: 10.1007/s10865-016-9782-2 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

10. Horne R, James D, Petrie K, Weinman J, Vincent R. Patients’ interpretation of symptoms as a cause of delay in reaching hospital during acute myocardial infarction. Heart. 2000;83(4):388–93. doi: 10.1136/heart.83.4.388 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

11. Ross S, Walker A, MacLeod MJ. Patient compliance in hypertension: role of illness perceptions and treatment beliefs. Journal of human hypertension. 2004;18(9):607–13. doi: 10.1038/sj.jhh.1001721 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

12. Lobban F, Barrowclough C, Jones S. A review of the role of illness models in severe mental illness. Clinical psychology review. 2003;23(2):171–96. doi: 10.1016/s0272-7358(02)00230-1 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

13. Kinderman P, Setzu E, Lobban F, Salmon P. Illness beliefs in schizophrenia. Social Science Medicine. 2006;63(7):1900–11. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.04.022 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

14. Williams B, Healy D. Perceptions of illness causation among new referrals to a community mental health team:“explanatory model” or “exploratory map”? Social science medicine. 2001;53(4):465–76. doi: 10.1016/s0277-9536(00)00349-x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

15. Petrie KJ, Broadbent E, Kydd R. Illness perceptions in mental health: Issues and potential applications. Taylor & Francis; 2008. [Google Scholar]

16. Clatworthy J, Bowskill R, Rank T, Parham R, Horne R. Adherence to medication in bipolar disorder: a qualitative study exploring the role of patients’ beliefs about the condition and its treatment. Bipolar disorders. 2007;9(6):656–64. doi: 10.1111/j.1399-5618.2007.00434.x [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

17. Pollack LE, Aponte M. Patients’ perceptions of their bipolar illness in a public hospital setting. Psychiatric Quarterly. 2001;72(2):167–79. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

18. Güner P. Illness perception in Turkish schizophrenia patients: a qualitative explorative study. Archives of psychiatric nursing/ 2014;28(6):405–12. doi: 10.1016/j.apnu.2014.08.016 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

19. Higbed L, Fox JR. Illness perceptions in anorexia nervosa: A qualitative investigation. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2010;49(3):307–25. doi: 10.1348/014466509X454598 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

20. Pedley R, Bee P, Wearden A, Berry KJPo. Illness perceptions in people with obsessive-compulsive disorder; A qualitative study. PloS One. 2019;14(3):e0213495. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0213495 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

21. Leventhal H, Diefenbach M, Leventhal EA. Illness cognition: using common sense to understand treatment adherence and affect cognition interactions. Cognitive Therapy Research. 1992;16(2):143–63. [Google Scholar]

22. Nakane Y, Jorm AF, Yoshioka K, Christensen H, Nakane H, Griffiths KM. Public beliefs about causes and risk factors for mental disorders: a comparison of Japan and Australia. BMC psychiatry. 2005;5(1):33. doi: 10.1186/1471-244X-5-33 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

23. Eisenberg L. Disease and illness distinctions between professional and popular ideas of sickness. Culture, medicine psychiatry. 1977;1(1):9–23. doi: 10.1007/BF00114808 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

24. Pols AD, Schipper K, Overkamp D, Van Marwijk HW, Van Tulder MW, Adriaanse MC. Patients’ and practice nurses’ perceptions of depression in patients with type 2 diabetes and/or coronary heart disease screened for subthreshold depression. BMC family practice. 2018;19(1):202. doi: 10.1186/s12875-018-0870-y [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

25. Moskowitz JT, Wrubel J, Hult JR, Maurer S, Acree M. Illness appraisals and depression in the first year after HIV diagnosis. Plos one. 2013;8(10):e78904. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0078904 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

26. Murray J, Banerjee S, Byng R, Tylee A, Bhugra D, Macdonald A. Primary care professionals’ perceptions of depression in older people: a qualitative study. Social science medicine.2006;63(5):1363–73. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2006.03.037 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

27. Elwy AR, Yeh J, Worcester J, Eisen SV. An illness perception model of primary care patients’ help seeking for depression. Qualitative health research. 2011;21(11):1495–507. doi: 10.1177/1049732311413781 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

28. Magaard JL, Löwe B, Brütt AL, Kohlmann S. Illness beliefs about depression among patients seeking depression care and patients seeking cardiac care: an exploratory analysis using a mixed method design. BMC psychiatry. 2018;18(1):366. doi: 10.1186/s12888-018-1936-z [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

29. Magaard JL, Schulz H, Brütt AL. What do patients think about the cause of their mental disorder? A qualitative and quantitative analysis of causal beliefs of mental disorder in inpatients in psychosomatic rehabilitation. PloS one. 2017;12(1):e0169387. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0169387 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

30. Organization WH. Depression and other common mental disorders: global health estimates. World Health Organization; 2017. [Google Scholar]

31. McGorry PD, Goldstone SD, Parker AG, Rickwood DJ, Hickie IB. Cultures for mental health care of young people: an Australian blueprint for reform. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2014;1(7):559–68. doi: 10.1016/S2215-0366(14)00082-0 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

32. Cheng YE. Cultural Politics of Education and Human Capital Formation: Learning to Labor in Singapore. In: Abebe T, Waters J, Skelton T, editors. Laboring and Learning. Singapore: Springer Singapore; 2017. p. 265–84. [Google Scholar]

33. Subramaniam M, Abdin E, Vaingankar J, Shafie S, Chua B, Sambasivam R, et al. Tracking the mental health of a nation: prevalence and correlates of mental disorders in the second Singapore mental health study. Epidemiology Psychiatric Sciences. 2020:1–10. doi: 10.1017/S2045796019000179 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

34. Subramaniam M, Abdin E, Vaingankar JA, Shafie S, Chua HC, Tan WM, et al. Minding the treatment gap: results of the Singapore Mental Health Study. Social Psychiatry and Psychiatric Epidemiology. 2019:1–10. doi: 10.1007/s00127-019-01748-0 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

35. McCabe R, Priebe S. Explanatory models of illness in schizophrenia: comparison of four ethnic groups. The British Journal of Psychiatry. 2004;185(1):25–30. doi: 10.1192/bjp.185.1.25 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

36. Chong SA, Abdin E, Vaingankar JA, Kwok KW, Subramaniam M. Where do people with mental disorders in Singapore go to for help? Ann Acad Med Singapore. 2017;41(4):154–60. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

37. Bishop GD. East meets West: Illness cognition and behaviour in Singapore. Applied Psychology. 1998;47(4). [Google Scholar]

38. Quah S-H, Bishop GD. Seeking help for illness: The roles of cultural orientation and illness cognition. Journal of Health Psychology. 1996;1(2):209–22. doi: 10.1177/135910539600100205 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

39. Nisbett R. The geography of thought: How Asians and Westerners think differently… and why: Simon and Schuster; 2004. [Google Scholar]

40. Braun V, Clarke V. Thematic analysis. 2012. [Google Scholar]

41. Braun V, Clarke V. Using thematic analysis in psychology. Qualitative research in psychology. 2006;3(2):77–101. [Google Scholar]

42. Corbin J, Strauss A. Basics of qualitative research: Techniques and procedures for developing grounded theory: Sage publications; 2014. [Google Scholar]

43. Strauss A, Corbin J. Basics of qualitative research techniques: Sage publications; Thousand Oaks, CA; 1998. [Google Scholar]

44. Fereday J, Muir-Cochrane E. Demonstrating rigor using thematic analysis: A hybrid approach of inductive and deductive coding and theme development. International journal of qualitative methods. 2006;5(1):80–92. [Google Scholar]

45. Williams G. The genesis of chronic illness: narrative re‐construction. Sociology of health illness. 1984;6(2):175–200. doi: 10.1111/1467-9566.ep10778250 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

46. Brown P. Naming and framing: The social construction of diagnosis and illness. Journal of health social behavior. 1995:34–52. [PubMed] [Google Scholar]

47. Feixas G, Montesano A, Erazo-Caicedo MI, Compañ V, Pucurull O. Implicative dilemmas and symptom severity in depression: a preliminary and content analysis study. Journal of Constructivist Psychology. 2014;27(1):31–40. [Google Scholar]

48. Chua B-H. Racial-Singaporeans: Absence after the hyphen. Social Scientist. 1996:51–68. [Google Scholar]

49. Reynolds J, Stewart M, MacDonald R, Sischo L. Have adolescents become too ambitious? High school seniors’ educational and occupational plans, 1976 to 2000. Social Problems. 2006;53(2):186–206. [Google Scholar]

50. Tan AC, Rehfuss MC, Suarez EC, Parks-Savage A. Nonsuicidal self-injury in an adolescent population in Singapore. Clinical child psychology psychiatry. 2014;19(1):58–76. doi: 10.1177/1359104512467273 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

51. Wrosch C, Miller GE. Depressive symptoms can be useful: Self-regulatory and emotional benefits of dysphoric mood in adolescence. Journal of Personality Social Psychology. 2009;96(6):1181. doi: 10.1037/a0015172 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

52. Wrosch C, Scheier MF, Carver CS, Schulz R. The importance of goal disengagement in adaptive self-regulation: When giving up is beneficial. Self Identity. 2003;2(1):1–20. [Google Scholar]

53. Dickson JM, Moberly NJ, O’Dea C, Field M. Goal fluency, pessimism and disengagement in depression. PloS one. 2016;11(11):e0166259. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0166259 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

54. Dickson JM, Moberly NJ. Reduced specificity of personal goals and explanations for goal attainment in major depression. PloS one. 2013;8(5):e64512. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0064512 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

55. Ang RP, Huan VS. Academic expectations stress inventory: Development, factor analysis, reliability, and validity. Educational Psychological Measurement. 2006;66(3):522–39. [Google Scholar]

56. Yi C-C. Adolescents and transition to adulthood in Asia 2015. 191–210 p. [Google Scholar]

57. Beck AT. Cognitive therapy and the emotional disorders: Penguin; 1979. [Google Scholar]

58. Feixas G, Bados A, García‐Grau E, Paz C, Montesano A, Compañ V, et al. A dilemma‐focused intervention for depression: A multicenter, randomized controlled trial with a 3‐month follow‐up. Depression and anxiety. 2016;33(9):862–9. doi: 10.1002/da.22510 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

59. Feixas G, Montesano A, Compan V, Salla M, Dada G, Pucurull O, et al. Cognitive conflicts in major depression: Between desired change and personal coherence. British Journal of Clinical Psychology. 2014;53(4):369–85. doi: 10.1111/bjc.12050 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

60. Chong SA, Abdin E, Vaingankar JA, Heng D, Sherbourne C, Yap M, et al. A population-based survey of mental disorders in Singapore. Ann Acad Med Singapore. 2017. [Google Scholar]

61. Klimstra TA, van Doeselaar L. Identity formation in adolescence and young adulthood. Personality development across the lifespan: Elsevier; 2017. p. 293–308. [Google Scholar]

62. Salmela-Aro K. Personal goals during the transition to young adulthood 2001. 59–84 p. [Google Scholar]

63. Freund AM, Hennecke M, Mustafic M. On gains and losses, means and ends: Goal orientation and goal focus across adulthood: The Oxford handbook of human motivation; 2012. [Google Scholar]

64. Newman BM, Newman PR. Development through life: A psychosocial approach: Cengage Learning; 2017. [Google Scholar]

65. Kaplan A, Flum H. Achievement goal orientations and identity formation styles. Educational Research Review. 2010;5(1):50–67. [Google Scholar]

66. Alsagoff L. English in Singapore: Culture, capital and identity in linguistic variation. World Englishes. 2010;29(3):336–48. [Google Scholar]

67. Haroz E, Ritchey M, Bass J, Kohrt B, Augustinavicius J, Michalopoulos L, et al. How is depression experienced around the world? A systematic review of qualitative literature. Social Science Medicine. 2017;183:151–62. doi: 10.1016/j.socscimed.2016.12.030 [PMC free article] [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

68. Subramaniam M, Abdin E, Jeyagurunathan A, Chang S, Samari E, Shafie S, et al. Exploration of illness perception among patients with mental illness in a multi-ethnic Asian sample. Psychiatry research. 2018;267:516–27. doi: 10.1016/j.psychres.2018.06.032 [PubMed] [CrossRef] [Google Scholar]

69. Benjamin J. Beyond doer and done to: Recognition theory, intersubjectivity and the third: Taylor & Francis; 2017. [Google Scholar]

70. Tew J, Ramon S, Slade M, Bird V, Melton J, Le Boutillier C. Social factors and recovery from mental health difficulties: a review of the evidence. The British Journal of Social Work. 2012;42(3):443–60. [Google Scholar]

71. Meehan J. 3. Recognition And The Dynamics Of Intersubjectivity. Axel Honneth: Critical Essays: Brill; 2011. p. 89–123. [Google Scholar]

Decision Letter 0

12 Oct 2020

PONE-D-20-07514

A reduced state of being: illness perceptions in young adults diagnosed with depressive disorders

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Teh,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by November 23, 2020. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at gro.solp@enosolp. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Stephan Doering, M.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal requirements:

When submitting your revision, we need you to address these additional requirements.

1. Please ensure that your manuscript meets PLOS ONE's style requirements, including those for file naming. The PLOS ONE style templates can be found at

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=wjVg/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_main_body.pdf and

https://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/file?id=ba62/PLOSOne_formatting_sample_title_authors_affiliations.pdf

2. Please provide further detail regarding the interviews that were conducted as part of this study.

In the Methods section please outline the topics covered in the interview.

Please state whether an interview guide was used. If so, please include a copy of the topic/interview guide used in the study, in both the original language and English, as Supporting Information, or include a citation if it has been published previously.

3. Please provide additional details regarding participant consent.

In the Methods section, please state why it was not possible to obtain written consent, how verbal consent was recorded and whether the ethics committee approved this consent procedure.

If your study included minors, state whether you obtained consent from parents or guardians.

4. We note that you have indicated that data from this study are available upon request. PLOS only allows data to be available upon request if there are legal or ethical restrictions on sharing data publicly. For more information on unacceptable data access restrictions, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-unacceptable-data-access-restrictions.

In your revised cover letter, please address the following prompts:

a) If there are ethical or legal restrictions on sharing a de-identified data set, please explain them in detail (e.g., data contain potentially sensitive information, data are owned by a third-party organization, etc.) and who has imposed them (e.g., an ethics committee). Please also provide contact information for a data access committee, ethics committee, or other institutional body to which data requests may be sent.

b) If there are no restrictions, please upload the minimal anonymized data set necessary to replicate your study findings as either Supporting Information files or to a stable, public repository and provide us with the relevant URLs, DOIs, or accession numbers. For a list of acceptable repositories, please see http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/data-availability#loc-recommended-repositories.

We will update your Data Availability statement on your behalf to reflect the information you provide.

5. Please amend your manuscript to include your abstract after the title page.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Partly

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

2. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: N/A

Reviewer #2: N/A

**********

3. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

4. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: This paper presents an original qualitative research on illness perceptions in young adults diagnosed with depressive disorders based on an Asian context (Singapour). Overall, the topic is highly relevant for psychology and the qualitative perspective s most appropriate to explore narratives to understand the lived experience of people with depressive disorders. The paper is well-structured and the English is correct.

Despite these qualities, the current version of the paper calls for substantial changes at epistemological, theoretical and methodological levels in order to be potentially ready for publication in PLOS ONE, especially given the solid and high quality of this journal. Here below my suggestions to improve the quality of the present version of the manuscript:

1) Introduction: The Leventhal model is presented in the introductory part as a core theoretical perspective mobilized by the authors, as they position themselves critically towards such model. However, the reader wonders why, given the broad body of qualitative research on depression, the introduction is narrowed down to research on this model? What about other qualitative approaches to depression, that also take into account narratives on illness (ex. phenomenology and IPA, narrative, discursive...).

I very much appreciated that you mention the role of culture as it shapes the experience of illness - This is an inherent part of the socio-constructionist perspective namely. But scientific references are missing to better argue on the crucial role of culture (understood as a set of norms and values - giving rise to social narratives that contribute to determine how we experience the world and ourselves). The distinction between West and East remains rather superficial and broad. What seems specific to the context you chose for your research and why is it relevant for the hypothesis that you raise at the end of this section?

Furthermore, while narratives seem the crucial focus on this paper, the reader cannot really grasp from the manuscript's content why the narrative focus is so important. As a qualitative researcher in psychology, I personally fully agree with you on the central role of narratives. But why are narratives so important? Different perspectives have been developed within psychology, namely that of structuring our sense of selves and our world (discursive, constructionist, narrative), but also that of making sense of our experience (for example, phenomenology). However, the choices underlying your theoretical approach seem absent /scarcely developed. Indeed, there are multiple approaches to narratives - and this theoretical foundation is currently missing from your paper. The problematisation at the end of the introduction falls short: why the hypothesis on the sense of self? Can you develop what you mean by this and argue with additional references to make your research/theoretical posture more explicit?

2) Methodology: To me, this section requires important changes. Under the 'procedure' paragraph, the population is already mentioned but then you come back to participants in the next paragraph entitled 'participants' - The reader can be a bit lost in this order. I would suggest to better justify the qualitative approach used in the 'procedure' paragraph to better explain the whole methodological procedure in a general way.

line 119: what do you mean by 'subconsciously'? not appropriate from my perspective - better explain your decision on the context of the interviews

In the population paragraph, the average age needs to be better justified - (especially if 'young adults' is part of your title - why young adults rather than older adults? Also, there is no mention of the gender except for the number of male and female - but according to me, the 'gender' dimension needs to be at least considered and justified to increase methodological rigor - as in depression this seems to play an important role (ex. constructions of masculinity/feminity)

Also, on the interview description - there is no information on the interview guide nor how it was elaborated. This is an important aspect to better understand how the analysis were conducted afterwards, in the analytical phase.

Regarding the 'analysis' section - practically no scientific references are used to justify your analysis technique. Yet, this methodological stage is particularly important. I would suggest reading Virginia Braun and Victoria Clarke who have extensively written on thematic content analysis.

Line 144: Saturation? Better explain and use scientific reference (This notion is contested in qualitative research depending on the authors).

Line 158: open and ground coding - what does this mean? Reference please

3) Results: The themes are very general - A 'good' theme definition is that which captures in its name the full condensed 'story' or 'message' that the given set of data portrays.

Moreover, please restructure with numbers or letters in hierarchical order the different themes and subthemes as it is difficult for the reader to follow. I regret that there is a lot of 'verbatim' that illustrate the names of your subthemes but little explanation or interpretation on what these themes and subthemes mean in relation to your hypothesis and research aim. At times, the text is written as if narratives 'reflected' a 'truth' or a 'reality' - Please keep in mind what status do narratives have in your theoretical perspective to better guide you on how to write about them.

I sometimes disagreed with the authors' explanation on certain quotes - to me, Line 200 for instance, would call for a separation between mind and body rather than depression as a different entity, from what I understand from the participants' verbatim

Often, authors refer to results as 'they' (participants') as if all participants had positioned themselves in an homogeneous way regarding their experience. Were there any dissident narratives for example?

Line 286: while this seems a key theme (or subtheme?) it is very little developed/explained.

Line 315: spelling of 'yeah'

Line 352: This theme follows a hierarchical numeration while the rest of the themes do not.

In the end of the result section, a synthesis of results would be very useful.

Discussion: Coming back to the CSM model - why and how it was confirmed? Why do authors overlook the similarities of their results with the model (if it's an important focus of the paper)?

Line 428 'are under pressure' - this is a strong affirmation that is different from 'perceive themselves as being under strong pressure' for example. This study is qualitative so it takes the subjective constructions into account - not revealing an 'objective' reality.

Line 474: regarding the limitations, please consider that the aim of qualitative research is not generalization but on the contrary, considering variablity, contextualization, narratives, singularity, experience - therefore it is not a limitation from my view.

Finally, how can your conclusions be extended beyond the Leventhal model and contribute to psychology more broadly? This could be a stronger case if your theoretical position/framework were better made explicit in the introduction for example.

Reviewer #2: Thank you for asking me to review this manuscript.

This qualitative inquiry aimed to explore the illness perceptions of Chinese, Malay, and Indian young adults diagnosed with depressive disorders by using one-to one face-to-face semi-structured interviews. They concluded that depression was typically experienced as a reduced state of being, and was thought of cognitively as an entity that may be a part of or separate from the self. Five themes were identified as: 1) meanings, 2) causes, 3) symptoms, 4) consequences, and 5) chronicity of depression.

Such an effort could be valuable for cultural diversity and future implication. The following issues need to be considered:

1. In abstract, the research purpose is expected to be added.

2. In abstract, Conclusion-“Depression was typically experienced as a reduced state of being, and was thought of cognitively as an entity that may be a part of or separate from the self. .. the results emphasized the importance of examining self concepts in therapy and recovery.…” Such a conclusion might be relevant to young adults’ developmental task at their developmental stage? It might not be applicable for other groups at different ages?

3. In Introduction, please add the rationale why selecting the young adults as a target? Relevant research significance would be helpful, eg; What’s the global prevalence of major depression in young-aged papulation?

4. The positive aspects of the illness perception were mentioned from several literatures (Line 48-57). Please compare your results with previous studies in Discussion.

5. In Method, Line121-More clear information is needed, for example: interviewees’ qualification and training? Were interview process standardized or using the identical interview guidelines? What contents of interview guidelines included? Or how did you achieve the same focus of the interview contents?

6. How could the researcher prevent yourselves to be influenced or guided by these prior understanding? For example, the existing knowledge on the Common-sense Model of Self-Regulation (CSM) by Leventhal. Was the interview guideline or analytical coding process followed by the CSM theory?

7. Line144-150, please add the Standardized Deviation following the mean to indicate the variation and range of the sample characteristics. Most are single, please report the %.

8. Line148, 6 participants live with depressive disorders lower than 1 year, with a mean of 3.5 years, and the least was 4 months. How will it influence your results? It might be some limitation.

9. About the analysis, in Line 145-146- How was the transcript of one individual from Sri Lankan analyzed with the rest of the transcripts as a whole?

10. In Results, it’s not easy to distinguish the “themes” and “sub-themes”. Please clearly separate the “5-themes” and “sub-themes” to increase the readability.

11. Among all themes identified as cognitive representations, I’m wondering was there any emotional perspective or anything related to their coping or management with the mood symptoms while looking at their illness ration than cognitive aspects?

12. Line 356, “3.4.1…”?

13. Table—what’s the meaning of the 「^」 following the number, eg: 2^ or 7^?

**********

6. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: Yes: Maria del Rio Carral

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at gro.solp@serugif. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Author response to Decision Letter 0

25 Nov 2020

18 November 2020

RE: Responses to reviewers

Dear Reviewers,

We thank you for taking the time to review our manuscript. We have learnt a lot from the peer review process and have incorporated your comments in our next draft. Please find our point-by-point responses to the comments in the boxed text area below:

Reviewer #1: This paper presents an original qualitative research on illness perceptions in young adults diagnosed with depressive disorders based on an Asian context (Singapour). Overall, the topic is highly relevant for psychology and the qualitative perspective s most appropriate to explore narratives to understand the lived experience of people with depressive disorders. The paper is well-structured and the English is correct.

Despite these qualities, the current version of the paper calls for substantial changes at epistemological, theoretical and methodological levels in order to be potentially ready for publication in PLOS ONE, especially given the solid and high quality of this journal. Here below my suggestions to improve the quality of the present version of the manuscript:

1) Introduction: The Leventhal model is presented in the introductory part as a core theoretical perspective mobilized by the authors, as they position themselves critically towards such model. However, the reader wonders why, given the broad body of qualitative research on depression, the introduction is narrowed down to research on this model? What about other qualitative approaches to depression, that also take into account narratives on illness (ex. phenomenology and IPA, narrative, discursive...).

Answer: Thank you for this. We agree with the reviewer that more could be elaborated on this point. There are important reasons for using Leventhal’s model. Firstly, the Leventhal model is one of the most widely used model due to its good psychometric properties, to predict self-care behaviours and recovery in physical illnesses. Secondly, unlike other health belief models, this model comprises of emotional illness representations, which are directly relevant to mental illnesses. We have elaborated on this point in the subsequent draft.

We agree that there are many other qualitative approaches that are highly relevant to the study of narratives. For instance, IPA is very useful in providing rich accounts of the depression experience. However, as it had been previously discussed by Braun and Clarke (2020) and Chamberlain (2012), there is no one ideal or superior qualitative method. The choice of method may also be dependent on pragmatic factors as well- we are unfortunately limited in our access to resources and are facing time constraints. Finally, authors (and coders of this study) are experienced and trained in thematic analysis only. We wish to be able to use other qualitative approaches in the near future because of its relevance in the work that we do. We agree with the reviewer that this was insufficiently justified in the manuscript and have made substantial edits to it.

I very much appreciated that you mention the role of culture as it shapes the experience of illness - This is an inherent part of the socio-constructionist perspective namely. But scientific references are missing to better argue on the crucial role of culture (understood as a set of norms and values - giving rise to social narratives that contribute to determine how we experience the world and ourselves). The distinction between West and East remains rather superficial and broad. What seems specific to the context you chose for your research and why is it relevant for the hypothesis that you raise at the end of this section?

Answer: Thank you for this. As the reviewer had succinctly pointed out, the illness experience of an individual may be influenced by societal norms and values. We agree fully with the reviewer that this portion is underdeveloped in the present draft, and have since made changes in the subsequent draft.

Furthermore, while narratives seem the crucial focus on this paper, the reader cannot really grasp from the manuscript's content why the narrative focus is so important. As a qualitative researcher in psychology, I personally fully agree with you on the central role of narratives. But why are narratives so important? Different perspectives have been developed within psychology, namely that of structuring our sense of selves and our world (discursive, constructionist, narrative), but also that of making sense of our experience (for example, phenomenology). However, the choices underlying your theoretical approach seem absent /scarcely developed. Indeed, there are multiple approaches to narratives - and this theoretical foundation is currently missing from your paper. The problematisation at the end of the introduction falls short: why the hypothesis on the sense of self? Can you develop what you mean by this and argue with additional references to make your research/theoretical posture more explicit?

Answer: As quantitative researchers by training, we are largely unfamiliar with the theoretical concepts that are widely used in sociology. However, social constructivism, as we recognize, is highly relevant to our study. Thus, we understand and agree fully with the reviewer that the introduction can be improved further by laying theoretical grounds for the study and we hope the subsequent draft reads better. We thank the reviewer for these suggestions.

2) Methodology: To me, this section requires important changes. Under the 'procedure' paragraph, the population is already mentioned but then you come back to participants in the next paragraph entitled 'participants' - The reader can be a bit lost in this order. I would suggest to better justify the qualitative approach used in the 'procedure' paragraph to better explain the whole methodological procedure in a general way.

line 119: what do you mean by 'subconsciously'? not appropriate from my perspective - better explain your decision on the context of the interviews

Answer: Thank you for this. The main reason for carrying out the interviews away from the clinical setting was to prevent contextual priming at a subconscious level that may influence the way participants describe their illness experience clinically during the interviews. We have rephrased this explanation and restructured the paragraphs.

In the population paragraph, the average age needs to be better justified - (especially if 'young adults' is part of your title - why young adults rather than older adults? Also, there is no mention of the gender except for the number of male and female - but according to me, the 'gender' dimension needs to be at least considered and justified to increase methodological rigor - as in depression this seems to play an important role (ex. constructions of masculinity/feminity)

Answer: Major Depressive Disorder is the most prevalent mental illness among young adults aged 18 to 34 years old (Subramaniam et al., 2020), thus, these narratives describe how depression is typically experienced at the onset for this age group. Understanding how illness is experienced among young adults would be important for treatment and recovery that is also relevant for this age group.

Before the start of the study, researchers conducted a literature review of illness perception of severe mental illnesses. As reported in the introduction, the primary themes that emerged were related to the self, with no prominent mentions of gender. During the coding and analyses phase, the first author did suspect possible cultural influences on the role of gender. For instance, the theme “High societal expectations of success”, initially emerged from the narratives of male participants. This was detailed in her memo, indicating that instances like this could demonstrate cultural expectations of the need to succeed being more salient in men. However, during the course of coding, this theme was prominent in female participants as well. While we agree with the reviewer that gender does play an important role, it did not emerge during the literature review and analyses.

Also, on the interview description - there is no information on the interview guide nor how it was elaborated. This is an important aspect to better understand how the analysis were conducted afterwards, in the analytical phase.

Answer: Thank you for this, the interview guide has been attached as supporting information in the subsequent submission.

Regarding the 'analysis' section - practically no scientific references are used to justify your analysis technique. Yet, this methodological stage is particularly important. I would suggest reading Virginia Braun and Victoria Clarke who have extensively written on thematic content analysis. Line 144: Saturation? Better explain and use scientific reference (This notion is contested in qualitative research depending on the authors). Line 158: open and ground coding - what does this mean? Reference please

Answer: Thank you for this. We recognize that this is important but was mistakenly left out. We have uploaded the interview schedule in its final form. We have also improved on the analysis section to explain our sampling and analysis technique, including scientific references to support our justifications.

3) Results: The themes are very general - A 'good' theme definition is that which captures in its name the full condensed 'story' or 'message' that the given set of data portrays.

Moreover, please restructure with numbers or letters in hierarchical order the different themes and subthemes as it is difficult for the reader to follow. I regret that there is a lot of 'verbatim' that illustrate the names of your subthemes but little explanation or interpretation on what these themes and subthemes mean in relation to your hypothesis and research aim. At times, the text is written as if narratives 'reflected' a 'truth' or a 'reality' - Please keep in mind what status do narratives have in your theoretical perspective to better guide you on how to write about them.

Answer: Thank you for the suggestions. We have made additional changes to the name of the themes/subthemes to better represent the themes that emerged from the narratives.

I sometimes disagreed with the authors' explanation on certain quotes - to me, Line 200 for instance, would call for a separation between mind and body rather than depression as a different entity, from what I understand from the participants' verbatim

The verbatim quote may not have clearly captured the context. What we had understood from the participant during the interview was that “…the thing is in your mind. So [the thing in] the mind is taking over your body…” M11/31/F/M

Often, authors refer to results as 'they' (participants') as if all participants had positioned themselves in an homogeneous way regarding their experience. Were there any dissident narratives for example?

Answer: Thank you for this. Only the prominent themes were reported in the manuscript. The prominence of themes was not necessarily dependent on the majority although it was primarily so, and we would have stated if the themes or codes emerged only from a few. For instance, the narrative that depression is separate from or a part of the self, came from two different groups of participants, which we had merged to form a subtheme. We did encounter dissident narratives, for instance, narratives like, “depression comes from the brain so I think to actually rewire something takes a long time M12/26/F”, or “depression is something that’s very, very repetitive C12/22/F”, but we did not report as it was decided among coders that they were not prominent codes.

Line 286: while this seems a key theme (or subtheme?) it is very little developed/explained.

Line 315: spelling of 'yeah'

Line 352: This theme follows a hierarchical numeration while the rest of the themes do not.

In the end of the result section, a synthesis of results would be very useful.

Answer: Thanks for highlighting as these were mistakes. We have edited/removed them in the subsequent draft. The format of the manuscript has changed, and we hope it will read better.

Discussion: Coming back to the CSM model - why and how it was confirmed? Why do authors overlook the similarities of their results with the model (if it's an important focus of the paper)?

Thank you for this. The main objective of the study is to understand illness perceptions of depressive disorders, and it was not to disprove or confirm the CSM model. We had however, made few concluding statements as our study provided support for the conclusion that the CSM may be overly simplistic, although it is a useful guide for mental illnesses.

Line 428 'are under pressure' - this is a strong affirmation that is different from 'perceive themselves as being under strong pressure' for example. This study is qualitative so it takes the subjective constructions into account - not revealing an 'objective' reality.

Line 474: regarding the limitations, please consider that the aim of qualitative research is not generalization but on the contrary, considering variablity, contextualization, narratives, singularity, experience - therefore it is not a limitation from my view.

Answer: We agree with the reviewer and had removed this statement from limitations. We thank the reviewer for this important point.

Finally, how can your conclusions be extended beyond the Leventhal model and contribute to psychology more broadly? This could be a stronger case if your theoretical position/framework were better made explicit in the introduction for example.

Answer: The themes that emerged in this study were highly associated with the self, emphasizing the importance of self-concepts, such as self-agency, goals, sense of meaning and belonging, in the study of illness perceptions of severe mental illnesses. Additionally, our results evidently show that personal recovery requires the consideration of cultural beliefs that tie closely to illness beliefs among those surviving with depressive disorders in Singapore.

Thank you for taking the time to review this article. We hope the subsequent draft reads better.

Reviewer #2: Thank you for asking me to review this manuscript.

This qualitative inquiry aimed to explore the illness perceptions of Chinese, Malay, and Indian young adults diagnosed with depressive disorders by using one-to one face-to-face semi-structured interviews. They concluded that depression was typically experienced as a reduced state of being, and was thought of cognitively as an entity that may be a part of or separate from the self. Five themes were identified as: 1) meanings, 2) causes, 3) symptoms, 4) consequences, and 5) chronicity of depression.

Such an effort could be valuable for cultural diversity and future implication. The following issues need to be considered:

1. In abstract, the research purpose is expected to be added.

Answer: It has been added. We thank the reviewer for spotting this missing point.

2. In abstract, Conclusion-“Depression was typically experienced as a reduced state of being, and was thought of cognitively as an entity that may be a part of or separate from the self. .. the results emphasized the importance of examining self-concepts in therapy and recovery.…” Such a conclusion might be relevant to young adults’ developmental task at their developmental stage? It might not be applicable for other groups at different ages?

Answer: Thank you for this. We agree with the reviewer that individuals go through different developmental stages throughout their lives and the conclusion is only relevant to young adults, which is the premise of this study. Major Depressive Disorder is the most prevalent mental illness among young adults aged 18 to 34 years old (Subramaniam et al., 2020), thus, these narratives are descriptive of how depression is typically experienced at the onset for this age group. This aspect of the abstract has been changed to elaborate this point.

3. In Introduction, please add the rationale why selecting the young adults as a target? Relevant research significance would be helpful, eg; What’s the global prevalence of major depression in young-aged papulation?

Answer: Thank you for this. Local data reports that the onset of depressive disorders lie within the age range of 18-34 years, thereby making it a relevant age range to target. By understanding how depression is experienced among young adults, it may help to inform treatment and therapy that is relevant for this age group. We agree with the reviewer that this is underdeveloped in the original draft, and have made substantial improvements. We hope the subsequent draft reads better.

4. The positive aspects of the illness perception were mentioned from several literatures (Line 48-57). Please compare your results with previous studies in Discussion.

Answer: Thank you for this. Similarly seen in past reports on anorexia nervosa and obsessive compulsive disorder, depression was not always appraised as negative (Higbed & Fox, 2010; Pedley, Bee, Wearden, & Berry, 2019)). Having depression gave new meaning and perspectives (i.e. advocacy, empathy) into the lives of some participants. We agree with the reviewer that this portion should be included in the discussion.

5. In Method, Line121-More clear information is needed, for example: interviewees’ qualification and training? Were interview process standardized or using the identical interview guidelines? What contents of interview guidelines included? Or how did you achieve the same focus of the interview contents?

Answer: Thank you for this. The interviewers and coders were trained in thematic analysis conducted by the National University of Singapore (NUS). The interview process was standardized using an interview schedule, which we had left out during the first submission; we have included the interview guide as supporting information in the subsequent draft.

6. How could the researcher prevent yourselves to be influenced or guided by these prior understanding? For example, the existing knowledge on the Common-sense Model of Self-Regulation (CSM) by Leventhal. Was the interview guideline or analytical coding process followed by the CSM theory?

Answer: Thank you for this. Before the start of qualitative inquiry, the first author and coders conducted preliminary literature review searches to attain a level of theoretical sensitivity (Mills, Bonner, & Francis, 2006). The interview guide thus follows the CSM theory.

7. Line144-150, please add the Standardized Deviation following the mean to indicate the variation and range of the sample characteristics. Most are single, please report the %.

Answer: Thank you for this. We have reported the SD and percentages in the following draft.

8. Line148, 6 participants live with depressive disorders lower than 1 year, with a mean of 3.5 years, and the least was 4 months. How will it influence your results? It might be some limitation.

Answer: Thank you for the question. We understand the reviewer’s concerns: an individual who was officially diagnosed just 4 months ago would have a different experience than another who had been officially diagnosed 16 years ago. In this study, outpatients were invited to participate regardless of the length of years diagnosed. We did not put limits on this, as deciphering the exact start points of mental illness (unlike physical illness which are more clear) is difficult unless individuals were followed through a prospective longitudinal study. Furthermore, it is not uncommon for individuals to experience a duration of untreated depression for a good number of years without their awareness, before an official diagnosis is given. Thus, this information may be obscure as well. Finally, we did not find any deviations in narratives of those who lived with depressive disorders for less than 1 year than the rest.

9. About the analysis, in Line 145-146- How was the transcript of one individual from Sri Lankan analyzed with the rest of the transcripts as a whole?

Answer: That participant, though Sri Lankan, had mentioned in the interview (and recorded in the transcript) that she had “lived in Singapore most of (her) life”. Her narrative was not substantially different from the rest, and hence, her transcript was analysed as a whole since she did not belong to any of the major ethnic groups. That is, during the meetings between coders, the themes that were coded in her transcript was analysed and reviewed together with the rest of the transcripts while ignoring her Sri Lankan ethnicity.

10. In Results, it’s not easy to distinguish the “themes” and “sub-themes”. Please clearly separate the “5-themes” and “sub-themes” to increase the readability.

11. Among all themes identified as cognitive representations, I’m wondering was there any emotional perspective or anything related to their coping or management with the mood symptoms while looking at their illness ration than cognitive aspects?

Answer: Thank you for this, questions on coping methods and management were indeed asked during the interviews (please see the interview schedule). However, we had decided to discuss this portion in a separate manuscript.

12. Line 356, “3.4.1…”?

Answer: This was an error. Thank you for spotting this.

13. Table—what’s the meaning of the 「^」 following the number, eg: 2^ or 7^?

The symbol “^” denotes self-reported approximate number of years diagnosed with depressive disorder. These participants could not remember the exact year they were diagnosed, hence, the figures reported are a rough estimation.

Answer: Thank you very much for taking the time to review this article.

--

We hope the subsequent draft reads better and we are open for further discussions and feedback. We hope to hear from you soon and thank you very much again for the time to review our manuscript.

Your sincerely,

Wen Lin

Institute of Mental Health Singapore

References cited:

Higbed, L., & Fox, J. R. (2010). Illness perceptions in anorexia nervosa: A qualitative investigation. British Journal of Clinical Psychology, 49(3), 307-325.

Mills, J., Bonner, A., & Francis, K. (2006). The development of constructivist grounded theory. International journal of qualitative methods, 5(1), 25-35.

Pedley, R., Bee, P., Wearden, A., & Berry, K. (2019). Illness perceptions in people with obsessive-compulsive disorder; A qualitative study. PloS One, 14(3), e0213495.

Subramaniam, M., Abdin, E., Vaingankar, J., Shafie, S., Chua, B., Sambasivam, R., . . . Chua, H. (2020). Tracking the mental health of a nation: prevalence and correlates of mental disorders in the second Singapore mental health study. Epidemiology Psychiatric Sciences, 1-10.

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 1

5 Feb 2021

PONE-D-20-07514R1

A reduced state of being: illness perceptions in young adults diagnosed with depressive disorders

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr.Teh,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

Please submit your revised manuscript by March 31, 2021. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at gro.solp@enosolp. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Stephan Doering, M.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #1: (No Response)

Reviewer #2: (No Response)

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #1: N/A

Reviewer #2: N/A

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #1: Yes

Reviewer #2: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #1: Thank you for responding to my comments. While the new version is substantially better, in my view there are still important aspects that are problematic, from an epistemological point of view. In addition, I still have some concerns on the compatibility of theory (based on a model stemming from a rather cognitive tradition) and methodology (based on understanding of contextualized experiences). Last, the discussion remains too narrow (scientific and practical implications are scarcely discussed).

General comment on epistemological concerns

The use of qualitative approaches in psychology does not imply the ‘mere’ implementation of interviews + conducting a thematic content analysis, but rather requires an in-depth reflection on how personal experiences are shaped by the social and the cultural, as well as more psychological and embodied aspects. In order to analyse individuals’ experiences, qualitative researches need to de-construct certain presuppositions (e.g. when you state ‘Since illness beliefs are personal in nature’ – this is a very strong assumption that many qualitative psychologists would argue against – as many approaches show how individual experiences and subjectivity – including ‘beliefs’ are socially constructed and therefore contextual – thus the opposite of ‘personal in nature’)

Another example of ‘lack of coherence’ between incompatible paradigms is the following statement:

“From the perspectives of personality psychology and evolutionary biology, depressive symptoms are adaptive forms of coping that facilitates the detachment of existing unattainable goals to conserve resources which may otherwise further deplete such resources if individuals persisted with such goals (28-30).”

This purpose is not coherent with qualitative approaches as it ‘essentialises’ goal setting as if this skill was completely detached from social contexts, social discourses, etc.

General comment on the articulation of theory and qualitative methodology

As I have tried to explain, qualitative research tradition in psychology stresses the social embeddedness of psychological phenomena. Also, it requires to overcome the risk of methodolatry (see Chamberlain’s paper on this). There shall be more reasons than ‘simply’ practical or resource-related, to justify the use of a method rather than another one. Moreover, the difference that you state between interviews and focus groups is not necessarily true – many authors would argue that this statement

“Since illness beliefs are personal in nature one-to-one interviews are preferred over

focus group discussions as they provide the space to discuss personal attitudes without being pressured to give socially desirable answers” is not the difference between these two methods at all. As authors admit they come from more quantitative traditions, I would urge them to read qualitative research handbooks on these methods of data collection (and foremost, data analysis methods as they scarcely quote any authors in their analysis technique description)

General comment on scientific/broader implications

The discussion remains descriptive and too narrow – the reader may ask her/himself ‘so what?’ – what are the implications of studying individual experiences of depression in Singapour? As I had suggested in my previous review, the paper would improve its potential if it could broaden its scope from a theoretical perspective (What do your findings mean for theory on depression (Western-based) and furthermore, what about more practical implications for people who suffer from depression, for healthcare, for psychologists – namely in non Western contexts like Singapour.

Reviewer #2: PONE-D-20-07514 A reduced state of being: illness perceptions in young adults diagnosed with depressive disorders

Thank you for asking me to secondly review this manuscript. Most of the comments has been well responded, but the following issues still need to be considered to make their effort valuable in cultural diversity and future implication.

1. I’m curious could it be possible that the illness perceptions and experience of your participants have not limited to the CSM framework? The author replied that their interview guide follows the CSM theory, that will easily produce an accordant result with the CSM as their prior framework. How could the researcher prevent yourselves to be mainly influenced by the prior understanding on the existing knowledge on the CSM?

2. The stigma-related issues in non-western society have been mentioned in Discussion; but it has not been reflected from any of patients’ quotations?

3. Please clarify some unclear wordings in Discussion, such as:

…that was heavily instilled “from young” at home.

…the need to further understand depression in relation to goal failure and coping cannot be better emphasized by the temporal overlap.

4. I appreciate the revision has added more quotations to reveal the cultural significance, for example: “High societal expectations of success”, and “Unable to pursue personal goals that were incongruent with familial and societal expectations”. However, would it possibly relate to the families and the society’s insufficient mental health literacy about depression? It’s suggested to refer existing evidence on the family illness perception and stigma as follows. Please add some related implication as well.

Huang CH, Li SM, & Shu BC. Exploring the relationship between illness perceptions and negative emotions in relatives of people with schizophrenia within the context of an affiliate stigma model. J. Nurs. Res 24(3), 217- 223 (2016).

Lee, S. K., Lin, E. C. L., Chang, Y. F., Shao, W. C., & Lu, R. B. (2017). Psychometric evaluation of family illness perceptions of patients with schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatry, 7(7), 739-747.

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #1: No

Reviewer #2: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at gro.solp@serugif. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Attachment

Submitted filename: plos one review-20210111.docx

Author response to Decision Letter 1

10 Mar 2021

Dear Reviewers,

Thank you for taking the time to review our manuscript the second time. We have incorporated your comments and suggestions in our next draft. In addition, please find our point-by-point responses to your comments in the boxed text area below:

--

Reviewer #1: Thank you for responding to my comments. While the new version is substantially better, in my view there are still important aspects that are problematic, from an epistemological point of view. In addition, I still have some concerns on the compatibility of theory (based on a model stemming from a rather cognitive tradition) and methodology (based on understanding of contextualized experiences). Last, the discussion remains too narrow (scientific and practical implications are scarcely discussed).

General comment on epistemological concerns

The use of qualitative approaches in psychology does not imply the ‘mere’ implementation of interviews + conducting a thematic content analysis, but rather requires an in-depth reflection on how personal experiences are shaped by the social and the cultural, as well as more psychological and embodied aspects. In order to analyse individuals’ experiences, qualitative researches need to de-construct certain presuppositions (e.g. when you state ‘Since illness beliefs are personal in nature’ – this is a very strong assumption that many qualitative psychologists would argue against – as many approaches show how individual experiences and subjectivity – including ‘beliefs’ are socially constructed and therefore contextual – thus the opposite of ‘personal in nature’)

Reply: Perhaps a better way to explain is that individuals from collectivistic cultures have different communication styles than individualistic cultures. This has been rewritten in the next draft as we understand the possible conflict it brings theoretically.

Another example of ‘lack of coherence’ between incompatible paradigms is the following statement: “From the perspectives of personality psychology and evolutionary biology, depressive symptoms are adaptive forms of coping that facilitates the detachment of existing unattainable goals to conserve resources which may otherwise further deplete such resources if individuals persisted with such goals (28-30).” This purpose is not coherent with qualitative approaches as it ‘essentialises’ goal setting as if this skill was completely detached from social contexts, social discourses, etc.

Reply: We agree with the reviewer that this argument is inconsistent with the social constructivist perspective. Therefore, we have removed parts of the discussion section that were incongruent.

General comment on the articulation of theory and qualitative methodology

As I have tried to explain, qualitative research tradition in psychology stresses the social embeddedness of psychological phenomena. Also, it requires to overcome the risk of methodolatry (see Chamberlain’s paper on this). There shall be more reasons than ‘simply’ practical or resource-related, to justify the use of a method rather than another one. Moreover, the difference that you state between interviews and focus groups is not necessarily true – many authors would argue that this statement

“Since illness beliefs are personal in nature one-to-one interviews are preferred over

focus group discussions as they provide the space to discuss personal attitudes without being pressured to give socially desirable answers” is not the difference between these two methods at all. As authors admit they come from more quantitative traditions, I would urge them to read qualitative research handbooks on these methods of data collection (and foremost, data analysis methods as they scarcely quote any authors in their analysis technique description)

Reply: In the previous and subsequent draft, we had cited Clarke and Braun, as well as the framework documented by Feredey and Muir-Cochrane as our primary texts. The article by Feredey and Muir-Cochrane describes the inductive coding technique by Boyatzis (1998). These are the two main texts that we have cited as we believe they are sufficient to inform and guide our thematic analyses.

General comment on scientific/broader implications

The discussion remains descriptive and too narrow – the reader may ask her/himself ‘so what?’ – what are the implications of studying individual experiences of depression in Singapour? As I had suggested in my previous review, the paper would improve its potential if it could broaden its scope from a theoretical perspective (What do your findings mean for theory on depression (Western-based) and furthermore, what about more practical implications for people who suffer from depression, for healthcare, for psychologists – namely in non-Western contexts like Singapour.

Reply: The discussion section has been revised extensively on this aspect as we agree it is still lacking in its current form. We believe the results will be useful in stressing the importance of the sociocultural context in the recovery of depressive disorders in young adults. We thank the reviewer for taking the time to review our manuscript.

--

Reviewer #2: PONE-D-20-07514 A reduced state of being: illness perceptions in young adults diagnosed with depressive disorders

Thank you for asking me to secondly review this manuscript. Most of the comments has been well responded, but the following issues still need to be considered to make their effort valuable in cultural diversity and future implication.

1. I’m curious could it be possible that the illness perceptions and experience of your participants have not limited to the CSM framework? The author replied that their interview guide follows the CSM theory, that will easily produce an accordant result with the CSM as their prior framework. How could the researcher prevent yourselves to be mainly influenced by the prior understanding on the existing knowledge on the CSM?\\

Reply: The reviewer is accurate in stating that we are influenced by the literature reviews and CSM framework. While we recognized we are unable to distance ourselves entirely from being influenced by the theory, we had exercised a level of reflexivity that we believed were sufficient. For instance, the first author had written memos detailing her immediate thoughts during the entire process which were discussed alongside the development of the codebook among team members. Additionally, some members were not involved in the initial conceptualization of the study and hence were not informed of the literature reviews or framework. Thus, they had approached the interviews and analyses as ‘blinded’ co-investigators. Finally, interviewers conducted debriefings with their note-takers (if any) about the initial feelings and findings that had surfaced during the interview.

2. The stigma-related issues in non-western society have been mentioned in Discussion; but it has not been reflected from any of patients’ quotations?

Reply: While the cultural dimension plays an important role in illness perceptions of mental illness, the themes were too extensive to be reported in this manuscript. Cultural expectations that were directly related to the individual and illness perceptions, such as high societal expectations of success, were reported in this manuscript, whereas themes (and quotes/codes) stemming from the family/society that was more related to stigma than illness perception, will be reported in a separate manuscript.

3. Please clarify some unclear wordings in Discussion, such as:

…that was heavily instilled “from young” at home.

…the need to further understand depression in relation to goal failure and coping cannot be better emphasized by the temporal overlap.

Reply: These portions have been edited to improve readability and we hope it reads better in the next draft. We thank the reviewer for highlighting these areas.

4. I appreciate the revision has added more quotations to reveal the cultural significance, for example: “High societal expectations of success”, and “Unable to pursue personal goals that were incongruent with familial and societal expectations”.

Reply: Thank you for the suggestion, we had included more quotes for these themes and subthemes.

However, would it possibly relate to the families and the society’s insufficient mental health literacy about depression? It’s suggested to refer existing evidence on the family illness perception and stigma as follows. Please add some related implication as well.

Huang CH, Li SM, & Shu BC. Exploring the relationship between illness perceptions and negative emotions in relatives of people with schizophrenia within the context of an affiliate stigma model. J. Nurs. Res 24(3), 217- 223 (2016).

Lee, S. K., Lin, E. C. L., Chang, Y. F., Shao, W. C., & Lu, R. B. (2017). Psychometric evaluation of family illness perceptions of patients with schizophrenia. Neuropsychiatry, 7(7), 739-747.

Reply: While we agree there is overlap between stigma and illness perception, we had decided to report the theme of stigma in a separate article since it is too extensive to be reported in a single manuscript. Most subthemes of stigma that we had found do not overlap with illness perceptions. For instance, a quote from participant C02 who is 20/Female/Chinese, “they (the family) believe in like those zodiac kind like maybe it’s your bad year. I don’t know. I really... I’m not like those, I’m not superstitious or like believe...I don’t believe in those Chinese, those tradition”, suggests that these religious attitudes that were maintained by the older generations were rejected by the participants. In this manuscript, we had reported only the specific cultural expectations that individuals with depression had articulated to have a direct contribution and relevance to the perception of their illness.

Both Huang et al. and Lee et al. examined stigma in relatives of patients with schizophrenia and found that stigma influenced their illness perception toward mental illness. These articles are important as they show how familial attitudes can implicate illness recovery. However, these articles report cross-sectional stigma from the perspective of family and thus may have few direct relevance. Next, mental health literacy and stigma in Taiwan and in Singapore are relatively similar (i.e.: Tonsing, K. N. (2018). A review of mental health literacy in Singapore. Social work in health care,57(1), 27-47.) and therefore we have noted these articles during the drafting of our other manuscript. We thank the reviewer for suggesting these articles as it is relevant to stigma, which we, as an organisation are also highly interested.

Thank you.

Warm regards,

Wen Lin

Attachment

Submitted filename: Response to Reviewers.docx

Decision Letter 2

3 May 2021

PONE-D-20-07514R2

A reduced state of being: illness perceptions in young adults diagnosed with depressive disorders

PLOS ONE

Dear Dr. Wen Lin Teh,

Thank you for submitting your manuscript to PLOS ONE. After careful consideration, we feel that it has merit but does not fully meet PLOS ONE’s publication criteria as it currently stands. Therefore, we invite you to submit a revised version of the manuscript that addresses the points raised during the review process.

As you will see, the reviewers give quite contradictory comments and recommendations. Unfortunately, we had to invite new reviewers for the revised version of your manuscript, this might explain some views different from thjose of the previous reviewers. I tend to follow reviewer 4 and not reviewer 3. However, I would like to ask you to implement as much of comments and suggestions of both reviewers.

Please submit your revised manuscript by June 14, 2021. If you will need more time than this to complete your revisions, please reply to this message or contact the journal office at gro.solp@enosolp. When you're ready to submit your revision, log on to https://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/ and select the 'Submissions Needing Revision' folder to locate your manuscript file.

Please include the following items when submitting your revised manuscript:

  • A rebuttal letter that responds to each point raised by the academic editor and reviewer(s). You should upload this letter as a separate file labeled 'Response to Reviewers'.

  • A marked-up copy of your manuscript that highlights changes made to the original version. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Revised Manuscript with Track Changes'.

  • An unmarked version of your revised paper without tracked changes. You should upload this as a separate file labeled 'Manuscript'.

If you would like to make changes to your financial disclosure, please include your updated statement in your cover letter. Guidelines for resubmitting your figure files are available below the reviewer comments at the end of this letter.

If applicable, we recommend that you deposit your laboratory protocols in protocols.io to enhance the reproducibility of your results. Protocols.io assigns your protocol its own identifier (DOI) so that it can be cited independently in the future. For instructions see: http://journals.plos.org/plosone/s/submission-guidelines#loc-laboratory-protocols. Additionally, PLOS ONE offers an option for publishing peer-reviewed Lab Protocol articles, which describe protocols hosted on protocols.io. Read more information on sharing protocols at https://plos.org/protocols?utm_medium=editorial-email&utm_source=authorletters&utm_campaign=protocols.

We look forward to receiving your revised manuscript.

Kind regards,

Stephan Doering, M.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Journal Requirements:

Please review your reference list to ensure that it is complete and correct. If you have cited papers that have been retracted, please include the rationale for doing so in the manuscript text, or remove these references and replace them with relevant current references. Any changes to the reference list should be mentioned in the rebuttal letter that accompanies your revised manuscript. If you need to cite a retracted article, indicate the article’s retracted status in the References list and also include a citation and full reference for the retraction notice.

Reviewers' comments:

Reviewer's Responses to Questions

Comments to the Author

1. If the authors have adequately addressed your comments raised in a previous round of review and you feel that this manuscript is now acceptable for publication, you may indicate that here to bypass the “Comments to the Author” section, enter your conflict of interest statement in the “Confidential to Editor” section, and submit your "Accept" recommendation.

Reviewer #3: (No Response)

Reviewer #4: All comments have been addressed

**********

2. Is the manuscript technically sound, and do the data support the conclusions?

The manuscript must describe a technically sound piece of scientific research with data that supports the conclusions. Experiments must have been conducted rigorously, with appropriate controls, replication, and sample sizes. The conclusions must be drawn appropriately based on the data presented.

Reviewer #3: Partly

Reviewer #4: Yes

**********

3. Has the statistical analysis been performed appropriately and rigorously?

Reviewer #3: N/A

Reviewer #4: Yes

**********

4. Have the authors made all data underlying the findings in their manuscript fully available?

The PLOS Data policy requires authors to make all data underlying the findings described in their manuscript fully available without restriction, with rare exception (please refer to the Data Availability Statement in the manuscript PDF file). The data should be provided as part of the manuscript or its supporting information, or deposited to a public repository. For example, in addition to summary statistics, the data points behind means, medians and variance measures should be available. If there are restrictions on publicly sharing data—e.g. participant privacy or use of data from a third party—those must be specified.

Reviewer #3: Yes

Reviewer #4: Yes

**********

5. Is the manuscript presented in an intelligible fashion and written in standard English?

PLOS ONE does not copyedit accepted manuscripts, so the language in submitted articles must be clear, correct, and unambiguous. Any typographical or grammatical errors should be corrected at revision, so please note any specific errors here.

Reviewer #3: Yes

Reviewer #4: Yes

**********

6. Review Comments to the Author

Please use the space provided to explain your answers to the questions above. You may also include additional comments for the author, including concerns about dual publication, research ethics, or publication ethics. (Please upload your review as an attachment if it exceeds 20,000 characters)

Reviewer #3: Thank you very much for the invitiation to review this interesting manuscript. I have noticed that both the previous review-ers as well as the authors have done a great job in improving the draft. However, I have tried to review the current version as an independent reviewer, and thus focussed my review on the manuscript rather than the previous comments.

The qualitative paper describes illness perceptions of Chinese, Malay and Indian young adults living in Singapure with depres-sive disorder. Using face-to-face semir-structured interviews, the authors concluded that participants experienced depres-sion as a reduced state of being. Further, five themes were extracted from thematic analysis. Both topic and study popula-tion seem of interest for the field, yet while I am not an expert in qualitative research and I did not review the first draft of the manuscript, I have some substantial issues with its current version - especially regarding its theoretical foundation and its overall scope.

Introduction

1.) In the introduction, the CSM is explained quite well. But the illness representations of mental illness are oversimpli-fied, and studies on bipolar disorder, schizophrenia and later anorexia nervosa combined to construct a narrative stating that “the boundary between mental illness and the self is far less clear [than in case of physical illnesses] and often intersects”. This argumentation completely neglects modern biopsychosocial approaches in medicine, foster-ing dualistic thinking. Second, egosyntonic and egodystonic concepts of different mental illnesses – including de-pression – have been discussed in clinical psychology for years.

2.) The overview of the existing qualitative literature on the topic also seems somewhat random. There are numerous qualitative studies on depression, for example, while the manuscript suggests that “the majority of qualitative work has been conducted amongst severe but less common forms of mental illness such as psychosis or schizophrenia”. This might be true with regard to illness representations, it is however important to be more precise with the re-search question you want to investigate in the manuscript, and what previous research you built your argumenta-tion upon.

3.) When I was reading the sentence “To the best of our knowledge, there has been no qualitative inquiry into illness perceptions of depression that is specific to a non-western psychiatric population residing in Singapore”, I felt like this is the main novelty the manuscript provides. However, reading the title as well as the abstract, I got a com-pletely different first impression of the manuscript’s aims and hypotheses. I think you should really focus on the main research question, rather than generalize qualitative results from a very specific population. For example, I found the description of the multi-ethnic Singapore population really interesting and as an important context factor for the interviews.

4.) I do not really understand from the introduction why you focused on young adults, apart from them being the “most pervasive” age group. I think a group of young people living in Singapor suffering from depression is a very in-teresting, yet also very specific group of patients. I think your work would benefit a lot from focusing on the particu-lar cultural and social insights you may get from this group, rather than trying to generalize the results on depres-sion (or even mental illness) as an entity.

5.) The first reseach question if far too general, and there are numerous (clinical) works on the illness perceptions in (young) persons with depression. As you are not able to answer it with your data, I would at least add “in Singapore” to it, and then elaborate on specific cultural influences in the second research question.

6.) I found the last paragraph in the introduction (your hypotheses?) to be very confusing, as you only give general statements rather than linking them to either your research questions or your data.

Methods

7.) Please state what “IMH” stand for.

8.) I am rather confued by the interviews taking place “at a convient place and time”, but then again in the research in-terview room in IMH “to minimize the possibility that participants may describe their illness beliefs in medical terms”. Also, you matched interviewers and patients by ethnicity, without providing any rational why. Further, in-clusion criteria included ability to read and speak English, yet interviews were only “primarily” conducted in English. While there might be many good and interesting reasons to conduct the qualitative interviews the way you did, from a scientific point of view it all sounds very arbitrary.

9.) I think you should provide some information on the interviewers as well (medical doctors, researchers, psycholo-gists?).

10.) Were there any other (methodological) reasons for using thematic analysis by Clarke and Braun rather than the au-thors being familiar with it?

Results

11.) I think this section would highly benefit from a figure or an additional numbering, though this is mainly a visual re-mark.

12.) At no point you mentioned the fact that only two of your participants were married. Did this emerge as a topic throughout the interviews? If not, this should be addressed oin the discussion?

Discussion

13.) In accordance to 12, you should elaborate further on the specifics of your study population.

14.) I found it interesting to read that depression is “a chronic condition with no means of full recovery”; was this a gen-eral statement or a point of view maintained by all interviewees?

15.) I enjoyed the explanations on the peculiarities of Singaporian culture. As you have a very special study population, I would be careful regarding over-generalizing your findings, though – especially regarding clinical implications and therapy. Accordingly, I would elaborate further on the weaknesses and limitations of the study .

Reviewer #4: PONE-D-20-07514R2. A reduced state of being: illness perceptions in young adults diagnosed with

depressive disorders

This is a very interesting paper handling illness perceptions in young adults in a relatively understudied populations, which makes that the paper contributes to the literature. The paper has been reviewed before and the reviewers provide valuable suggestions which have been answered appropriately by the authors. As a newly, more recent added reviewer I will not go into the points raised before. I really appreciated the wide range of quotes of the participants given in the result section to support the categories reported on.

Minor points:

The paper’s impact on cultural significance is not reflected in the title of the manuscript. I suggest to revise the title to make the paper easier findable for potential interested readers.

Abstract: …surviving with depressive disorders… sounds unfamiliar to me. Do you mean ..remitted .. here?

Ending the discussion with a paragraph on the limitations of the study is not elegant and makes it more difficult for the reader to find the take home message of the paper. Please end the discussion with a concluding paragraph reflecting on the research questions given in the introduction of the manuscript (so without including speculations that cannot be derived from the current results).

**********

7. PLOS authors have the option to publish the peer review history of their article (what does this mean?). If published, this will include your full peer review and any attached files.

If you choose “no”, your identity will remain anonymous but your review may still be made public.

Do you want your identity to be public for this peer review? For information about this choice, including consent withdrawal, please see our Privacy Policy.

Reviewer #3: No

Reviewer #4: No

[NOTE: If reviewer comments were submitted as an attachment file, they will be attached to this email and accessible via the submission site. Please log into your account, locate the manuscript record, and check for the action link "View Attachments". If this link does not appear, there are no attachment files.]

While revising your submission, please upload your figure files to the Preflight Analysis and Conversion Engine (PACE) digital diagnostic tool, https://pacev2.apexcovantage.com/. PACE helps ensure that figures meet PLOS requirements. To use PACE, you must first register as a user. Registration is free. Then, login and navigate to the UPLOAD tab, where you will find detailed instructions on how to use the tool. If you encounter any issues or have any questions when using PACE, please email PLOS at gro.solp@serugif. Please note that Supporting Information files do not need this step.

Author response to Decision Letter 2

16 May 2021

Reviewer #3: Thank you very much for the invitiation to review this interesting manuscript. I have noticed that both the previous review-ers as well as the authors have done a great job in improving the draft. However, I have tried to review the current version as an independent reviewer, and thus focussed my review on the manuscript rather than the previous comments.

The qualitative paper describes illness perceptions of Chinese, Malay and Indian young adults living in Singapure with depres-sive disorder. Using face-to-face semir-structured interviews, the authors concluded that participants experienced depres-sion as a reduced state of being. Further, five themes were extracted from thematic analysis. Both topic and study popula-tion seem of interest for the field, yet while I am not an expert in qualitative research and I did not review the first draft of the manuscript, I have some substantial issues with its current version - especially regarding its theoretical foundation and its overall scope.

Introduction

1.) In the introduction, the CSM is explained quite well. But the illness representations of mental illness are oversimpli-fied, and studies on bipolar disorder, schizophrenia and later anorexia nervosa combined to construct a narrative stating that “the boundary between mental illness and the self is far less clear [than in case of physical illnesses] and often intersects”. This argumentation completely neglects modern biopsychosocial approaches in medicine, foster-ing dualistic thinking. Second, egosyntonic and egodystonic concepts of different mental illnesses – including de-pression – have been discussed in clinical psychology for years.

Answer: Thank you for the comments. The biopsychosocial model explains complex individual-environment interactions in relation to illness. Research generally supports the biopsychosocial model as a dominant framework of today and its use have been widespread. Narrative or lay-person models are similar to the biopsychosocial model in that it informs the biopsychosocial aspects of illness. While the biopsychosocial model is commonly used to explain the causality of illness using a three pronged approach, the CSM is used to explain how illness is perceived by the patients themselves. In the narratives, young adults generally endorsed the biopsychosocial model of depression, however, they placed greater emphasis and salience on sociocultural elements. From a social constructivist perspective, sociocultural elements of depression may inform egosyntonic and egodystonic concepts that are relevant to a multi-cultural non-western setting, which can be highly applicable locally since existing models are predominantly western-based. Qualitative approaches thus allow for the voice of culture to be heard. We respectfully disagree with the reviewer indicating that the CSM neglects the biopsychosocial model completely. Instead, both models do overlap, and we believe that the current focus on narratives can inform, substantially, in areas that the biopsychosocial model overlooks, such as culture.

2.) The overview of the existing qualitative literature on the topic also seems somewhat random. There are numerous qualitative studies on depression, for example, while the manuscript suggests that “the majority of qualitative work has been conducted amongst severe but less common forms of mental illness such as psychosis or schizophrenia”. This might be true with regard to illness representations, it is however important to be more precise with the re-search question you want to investigate in the manuscript, and what previous research you built your argumenta-tion upon.

Answer: While it may be true that there are numerous qualitative studies that had explored depression in its broadest form, qualitative inquiry into illness narratives of depression in young adults in South-east Asia are few and far between. Most of existing research on illness perception (cited) have been documented in western communities, and the extent to which it is relatable to a multi-ethnic non-western setting may be questioned. While we did expect to find illness narratives to be highly associated with the self and self-concepts as reported by past research, our research questions (and of past research) remained broad rather than specific to capture cultural nuances. Prompts were made to improve the specificity of our interview schedules but they may depend very much on the direction and content of narratives.

3.) When I was reading the sentence “To the best of our knowledge, there has been no qualitative inquiry into illness perceptions of depression that is specific to a non-western psychiatric population residing in Singapore”, I felt like this is the main novelty the manuscript provides. However, reading the title as well as the abstract, I got a com-pletely different first impression of the manuscript’s aims and hypotheses. I think you should really focus on the main research question, rather than generalize qualitative results from a very specific population. For example, I found the description of the multi-ethnic Singapore population really interesting and as an important context factor for the interviews.

Answer: We agree with the reviewer that the existing title does not encapsulate the intention and results of our study. We have made further changes to the title to capture our work better.

4.) I do not really understand from the introduction why you focused on young adults, apart from them being the “most pervasive” age group. I think a group of young people living in Singapor suffering from depression is a very in-teresting, yet also very specific group of patients. I think your work would benefit a lot from focusing on the particu-lar cultural and social insights you may get from this group, rather than trying to generalize the results on depres-sion (or even mental illness) as an entity.

Answer: Young adulthood is arguably the period where an individual is most productive in life and from an economic standpoint, mental illness can pose a significant threat to productivity and disease burden. This is crucial for Singapore as human capital is considered an important asset. Thus, we have focused on young adults due to this overlap. We have since elaborated our study’s rationale in the next draft.

We have added 3 new references to support the claims regarding the importance of culture in treatment relevance for young adulthood, citation [31]: McGorry PD, Goldstone SD, Parker AG, Rickwood DJ, Hickie IB. Cultures for mental health care of young people: an Australian blueprint for reform. The Lancet Psychiatry. 2014;1(7):559-68;

Human capital in Singapore, citation [32] Cheng YE. Cultural Politics of Education and Human Capital Formation: Learning to Labor in Singapore. In: Abebe T, Waters J, Skelton T, editors. Laboring and Learning. Singapore: Springer Singapore; 2017. p. 265-84; and

Treatment gaps in Singapore pertaining to mental illness: citation [34] Subramaniam, M., Abdin, E., Vaingankar, J. A., Shafie, S., Chua, H. C., Tan, W. M., ... & Chong, S. A. (2019). Minding the treatment gap: results of the Singapore Mental Health Study. Social psychiatry and psychiatric epidemiology, 1-10.

5.) The first reseach question if far too general, and there are numerous (clinical) works on the illness perceptions in (young) persons with depression. As you are not able to answer it with your data, I would at least add “in Singapore” to it, and then elaborate on specific cultural influences in the second research question.

Answer: We have included the terms “Singapore”, and “locally” to be more specific, we thank the reviewer for the suggestions.

6.) I found the last paragraph in the introduction (your hypotheses?) to be very confusing, as you only give general statements rather than linking them to either your research questions or your data.

Answer: Thank you for spotting this, while we do expect to find similar themes that were reported in past literature, we refrained from the terms hypothesis testing as the nature of the study is exploratory.

Methods

7.) Please state what “IMH” stand for.

Answer: Thank you for spotting this. The IMH stands for the Institute of Mental Health, the only public state psychiatric hospital in Singapore.

8.) I am rather confued by the interviews taking place “at a convient place and time”, but then again in the research in-terview room in IMH “to minimize the possibility that participants may describe their illness beliefs in medical terms”. Also, you matched interviewers and patients by ethnicity, without providing any rational why. Further, in-clusion criteria included ability to read and speak English, yet interviews were only “primarily” conducted in English. While there might be many good and interesting reasons to conduct the qualitative interviews the way you did, from a scientific point of view it all sounds very arbitrary.

Answer: Thank you for noting the contradictions. By default, researchers conducted the interviews in the research rooms that were situated away from the clinic. However, if a participant is unwilling, we would interview the participant at a time and place that is convenient for the him/her. The interviews were conducted in English, but participants, as with all young adults in Singapore, are bilingual, therefore, code-switching to Mandarin, Malay, Tamil, or other languages may happen during the interview. This brings us to our final point on cultural sensitivity. Interviewees were matched with interviewers as much as possible to minimize potential barriers from the lack of cultural sensitivity that may present itself during the interview. We have made changes to the method section to clear this contradiction and we hope it reads better in the next draft.

9.) I think you should provide some information on the interviewers as well (medical doctors, researchers, psycholo-gists?).

Answer: The authors (WLT, ES, LC, RK, FD, SS) are all quantitative researchers formal training (degrees in psychology/sociology at least), and we have had since received professional training and are experienced in the use of thematic analysis. Thank you for this suggestion. We have state this in the next draft.

10.) Were there any other (methodological) reasons for using thematic analysis by Clarke and Braun rather than the au-thors being familiar with it?

Answer: Yes, we chose this method of analysis because of the advantages it has for researchers working in healthcare. Thematic analysis has several advantages that is favourable for us in overcoming methodological constraints, such as a lack of time, resources, and expertise that are required for many other qualitative approaches. We have included these reasons in the next draft.

Results

11.) I think this section would highly benefit from a figure or an additional numbering, though this is mainly a visual re-mark.

Answer: We have included additional numberings in this section and we hope it helps to read better in the next draft.

12.) At no point you mentioned the fact that only two of your participants were married. Did this emerge as a topic throughout the interviews? If not, this should be addressed oin the discussion?

Answer: Thank you for this comment. In Singapore, the median age at the time of first marriage is around 30 years old for men and 29 years old for women. As the average age of our participants is approximately 26 years old, it was expected that being married would not be a common attribute. Further, we did not find emergent or significant themes related specifically to being married in this study as only a few of whom were married. As only the prominent themes were discussed, we had not reported them.

Discussion

13.) In accordance to 12, you should elaborate further on the specifics of your study population.

Answer: The specifics of our study population are elaborated in the “Participants” subsection under “Methods”.

14.) I found it interesting to read that depression is “a chronic condition with no means of full recovery”; was this a gen-eral statement or a point of view maintained by all interviewees?

It was both. It was a general statement and point of view maintained by the majority of our interviewees regardless of ethnicity. They had initially hoped depression could be resolved in an finite period of time, but they had soon felt it was not the case.

15.) I enjoyed the explanations on the peculiarities of Singaporian culture. As you have a very special study population, I would be careful regarding over-generalizing your findings, though – especially regarding clinical implications and therapy. Accordingly, I would elaborate further on the weaknesses and limitations of the study .

Answer: Generalization is not the aim of qualitative research. As reviewer #1 had pointed out, “the aim of qualitative research is not generalization but on the contrary, considering variablity, contextualization, narratives, singularity, experience - therefore it is not a limitation from my view”, which we had agreed with.

Thank you for taking the time to review our manuscript.

--

Reviewer #4: PONE-D-20-07514R2. A reduced state of being: illness perceptions in young adults diagnosed with depressive disorders

This is a very interesting paper handling illness perceptions in young adults in a relatively understudied populations, which makes that the paper contributes to the literature. The paper has been reviewed before and the reviewers provide valuable suggestions which have been answered appropriately by the authors. As a newly, more recent added reviewer I will not go into the points raised before. I really appreciated the wide range of quotes of the participants given in the result section to support the categories reported on.

Minor points:

The paper’s impact on cultural significance is not reflected in the title of the manuscript. I suggest to revise the title to make the paper easier findable for potential interested readers.

Answer: Thank you for this suggestion. We have made slight changes to better encapsulate the significance of culture in this manuscript.

Abstract: …surviving with depressive disorders… sounds unfamiliar to me. Do you mean ..remitted .. here?

Answer: Yes, we do mean remitted and have changed that term.

Ending the discussion with a paragraph on the limitations of the study is not elegant and makes it more difficult for the reader to find the take home message of the paper. Please end the discussion with a concluding paragraph reflecting on the research questions given in the introduction of the manuscript (so without including speculations that cannot be derived from the current results).

Answer: We have added a paragraph to properly conclude and summarise the study’s aims and findings. Thank you for taking the time to review our manuscript.

--

Thank you.

Sincerely,

Wen Lin

Attachment

Submitted filename: Letter to Reviewers 3.docx

Decision Letter 3

26 May 2021

A reduced state of being: the role of culture in illness perceptions of young adults diagnosed with depressive disorders in Singapore

PONE-D-20-07514R3

Dear Dr. Teh,

We’re pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been judged scientifically suitable for publication and will be formally accepted for publication once it meets all outstanding technical requirements.

Within one week, you’ll receive an e-mail detailing the required amendments. When these have been addressed, you’ll receive a formal acceptance letter and your manuscript will be scheduled for publication.

An invoice for payment will follow shortly after the formal acceptance. To ensure an efficient process, please log into Editorial Manager at http://www.editorialmanager.com/pone/, click the 'Update My Information' link at the top of the page, and double check that your user information is up-to-date. If you have any billing related questions, please contact our Author Billing department directly at gro.solp@gnillibrohtua.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please notify them about your upcoming paper to help maximize its impact. If they’ll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team as soon as possible -- no later than 48 hours after receiving the formal acceptance. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information, please contact gro.solp@sserpeno.

Kind regards,

Stephan Doering, M.D.

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE

Acceptance letter

1 Jun 2021

PONE-D-20-07514R3

A reduced state of being: The role of culture in illness perceptions of young adults diagnosed with depressive disorders in Singapore

Dear Dr. Teh:

I'm pleased to inform you that your manuscript has been deemed suitable for publication in PLOS ONE. Congratulations! Your manuscript is now with our production department.

If your institution or institutions have a press office, please let them know about your upcoming paper now to help maximize its impact. If they'll be preparing press materials, please inform our press team within the next 48 hours. Your manuscript will remain under strict press embargo until 2 pm Eastern Time on the date of publication. For more information please contact gro.solp@sserpeno.

If we can help with anything else, please email us at gro.solp@enosolp.

Thank you for submitting your work to PLOS ONE and supporting open access.

Kind regards,

PLOS ONE Editorial Office Staff

on behalf of

Professor Stephan Doering

Academic Editor

PLOS ONE


Articles from PLOS ONE are provided here courtesy of PLOS